Working‐Memory Capacity and the Evolution of Modern Cognitive Potential

Tool use is the main database to track down behavioral developments in the archaeological record and thus human evolution. Working‐memory capacity and modern cognitive potential, however, are no simple and obvious characters in tool behavior. Coded in cognigrams, which allow a direct comparison, animal and human tool use can be examined for specific aspects of the working‐memory capacity. Detailed studies of tool behavior of wasps, sea otters, bottlenose dolphins, and chimpanzees are presented and compared with the manufacture and use of Oldowan tools and Lower Paleolithic spears. Although this shows a wide range of problem‐solution distances, problem solving in animals seems to be restricted to problem complexes for which a solution can be found in spatial and temporal vicinity. In human evolution, the complexity of tool behavior increases regarding the number of active foci managed at a time in an action, the number and diversity of operational steps in a problem‐solution complex, and the spatial and temporal frame in which solutions are sought. The results suggest a gradual development of the different aspects of a complex capacity instead of a late introduction of a closed phenomenon with only different facets.

[1]  J. Kitahara-Frisch The origin of secondary tools , 1993 .

[2]  Julian M. Pine,et al.  Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. , 2004 .

[3]  A. Belfer‐Cohen,et al.  Modernity, Enhanced Working Memory, and the Middle to Upper Paleolithic Record in the Levant , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[4]  G. Schaller,et al.  TOOL-USING BEHAVIOR OF THE CALIFORNIA SEA OTTER , 1964 .

[5]  G. Hunt,et al.  Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  J. Mann,et al.  Can environmental heterogeneity explain individual foraging variation in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.)? , 2007, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[7]  Francisco Aboitiz,et al.  The Phonological Loop , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[8]  H. Burbano,et al.  The Derived FOXP2 Variant of Modern Humans Was Shared with Neandertals , 2007, Current Biology.

[9]  S. Pääbo,et al.  Intra- and Interspecific Variation in Primate Gene Expression Patterns , 2002, Science.

[10]  C. Sanz,et al.  New Insights into Chimpanzees, Tools, and Termites from the Congo Basin , 2004, The American Naturalist.

[11]  Frederick L. Coolidge,et al.  Working Memory, its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking , 2005 .

[12]  C. Beaman,et al.  Working memory and working attention : What could possibly evolve ? , 2015 .

[13]  Y. Sugiyama,et al.  The formation of the brush-sticks: modification of chimpanzees or the by-product of folding? , 2005, Primates.

[14]  N. Conard 23 Cultural Evolution in Africa and Eurasia During the Middle and Late Pleistocene , 2006 .

[15]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .

[16]  E. Reuland Imagination, Planning, and Working Memory , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[17]  A. Baddeley The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  C. Sanz,et al.  Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. , 2007, Journal of human evolution.

[19]  Lyn Wadley,et al.  Compound‐Adhesive Manufacture as a Behavioral Proxy for Complex Cognition in the Middle Stone Age , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[20]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  Per Berggren,et al.  Sponge Carrying by Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use? , 2010 .

[22]  C. Marean,et al.  The Origin of Modern Human Behavior , 2003, Current Anthropology.

[23]  J. Pruetz,et al.  Savanna Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, Hunt with Tools , 2007, Current Biology.

[24]  A. Nowell Working Memory and the Speed of Life , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[25]  A. Baddeley Is working memory still working? , 2001, The American psychologist.

[26]  A. Brooks,et al.  The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. , 2000, Journal of human evolution.

[27]  J. Pelegrin,et al.  Chaines Opératoires: un outil pour le Préhistorien , 1988 .

[28]  M. Martín-Loeches Uses and Abuses of the Enhanced‐Working‐Memory Hypothesis in Explaining Modern Thinking , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[29]  A. Nowell Human evolution, language and mind: A psychological and archaeological inquiry , 1999 .

[30]  Ursula Baumer,et al.  High-tech in the middle Palaeolithic: Neandertal-manufactured pitch identified , 2001, European Journal of Archaeology.

[31]  Nathan Schlanger The ancient mind: Mindful technology: unleashing the chaîne opératoire for an archaeology of mind , 1994 .

[32]  P. Barnard From Executive Mechanisms Underlying Perception and Action to the Parallel Processing of Meaning , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[33]  M. Rossano Making Friends, Making Tools, and Making Symbols , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[34]  R. Klein,et al.  The dawn of human culture , 2002 .

[35]  H. Thieme Altpaläolithische Holzgeräte aus Schöningen, Lkr. Helmstedt: Bedeutsame Funde zur Kulturentwicklung des frühen Menschen. , 1999 .

[36]  Russell D Gray,et al.  The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  T. Wynn,et al.  Executive Functions of the Frontal Lobes and the Evolutionary Ascendancy of Homo Sapiens , 2001 .

[38]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Great Ape Societies: Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use , 1996 .

[39]  T. Plummer Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. , 2004, American journal of physical anthropology.

[40]  R. A. Hernandez-Aguilar,et al.  Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  J. Mann,et al.  Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  Frederick L. Coolidge,et al.  The effect of enhanced working memory on language , 2006 .

[43]  W. Köhler The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.

[44]  R. Engle Role of Working‐Memory Capacity in Cognitive Control , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[45]  F. d’Errico The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity , 2003 .

[46]  B. Beck Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals , 1980 .

[47]  Stephan Veil Die Nachbildung der Lanze von Lehringen - Experimente zur Holzbearbeitung im Mittelpaläolithikum , 1990 .

[48]  I. Davidson The Colonization of Australia and Its Adjacent Islands and the Evolution of Modern Cognition , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[49]  H. Thieme Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany , 1997, Nature.

[50]  Peter J. Nilssen,et al.  Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene , 2007, Nature.

[51]  Norbert Mercier,et al.  Bitumen as a hafting material on Middle Palaeolithic artefacts , 1996, Nature.

[52]  S. Mithen The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion and science , 1999 .

[53]  S. Gould,et al.  Exaptation—a Missing Term in the Science of Form , 1982, Paleobiology.

[54]  A. Baddeley Is working memory still working , 2001 .

[55]  C. R. Peters,et al.  Late Pliocene Homo and Hominid Land Use from Western Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania , 2003, Science.

[56]  P. Weldon,et al.  Unique form of tool-using in two gastropod molluscs (Trochidae) , 1975, Nature.

[57]  P. Mellars The impossible coincidence. A single‐species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe , 2005 .

[58]  E. Bruner Morphological Differences in the Parietal Lobes within the Human Genus , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[59]  M. Tomasello The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition , 2000 .

[60]  J. Reyss,et al.  A Levallois point embedded in the vertebra of a wild ass (Equus africanus): hafting, projectiles and Mousterian hunting weapons , 1999, Antiquity.

[61]  Stanley H. Ambrose,et al.  Coevolution of Composite‐Tool Technology, Constructive Memory, and Language , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[62]  C. Boesch,et al.  Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking , 1984, Primates.

[63]  M. Lombard Evidence of hunting and hafting during the Middle Stone Age at Sibidu Cave, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa: a multianalytical approach. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[64]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crows , 2002, Science.

[65]  Lawrence H. Keeley,et al.  A Reappraisal of the Clacton Spearpoint , 1977, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society.