Chromatic contrast sensitivity: the role of absolute threshold and gain constant in differences between the fovea and the periphery.

A model of foveal achromatic and chromatic sensitivity [Vision Res. 36, 1597 (1996)] was extended to the peripheral visual field. Threshold-versus-illuminance functions were analyzed to determine effects of eccentricity on absolute thresholds and gain constants of chromatic and luminance mechanisms. The resulting peripheral model successfully predicted peripheral contrast sensitivity as a function of wavelength, for both white and 500-nm backgrounds. We conclude that the short-wavelength-sensitive cone opponent mechanism may mediate thresholds in Sloan's notch in the normal periphery and that interpretation of reduced chromatic sensitivity in the periphery requires an explicit model of how eccentricity affects both the gain constant and the absolute threshold.

[1]  J. Pokorny,et al.  The effect of background luminance on cone sensitivity functions. , 1989, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[2]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Pigment tests evaluated by a model of chromatic discrimination. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[3]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Red-Green Cone Interactions in the Increment-Threshold Spectral Sensitivity of Primates , 1971, Science.

[4]  C. Johnson,et al.  The Glenn A. Fry Award Lecture. Early losses of visual function in glaucoma. , 1995, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[5]  A. Nagy,et al.  Red-green color discrimination in peripheral vision , 1993, Vision Research.

[6]  P. King-Smith,et al.  Luminance and opponent-color contributions to visual detection and adaptation and to temporal and spatial integration. , 1976, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[7]  A B Watson,et al.  Estimation of local spatial scale. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  R. Hess,et al.  Human peripheral spatial resolution for achromatic and chromatic stimuli: limits imposed by optical and retinal factors. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  Mary A. Johnson,et al.  Color vision in the peripheral retina. , 1986 .

[10]  A. Milam,et al.  Distribution and morphology of human cone photoreceptors stained with anti‐blue opsin , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[11]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Sensitivity to spatiotemporal colour contrast in the peripheral visual field , 1983, Vision Research.

[12]  P A Sample,et al.  Short-wavelength automated perimetry without lens density testing. , 1994, American journal of ophthalmology.

[13]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[14]  J. Pokorny,et al.  How surrounds affect chromaticity discrimination. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  W. Swanson,et al.  Extracting thresholds from noisy psychophysical data , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  G. Wyszecki,et al.  Color Science Concepts and Methods , 1982 .

[17]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Effects of chromatic adaptation on opponent interactions in monkey increment-threshold spectral-sensitivity functions. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[18]  R. Weinreb,et al.  Evidence for a variety of functional defects in glaucoma-suspect eyes. , 1994, Journal of glaucoma.

[19]  P. King-Smith,et al.  Detection of coloured stimuli by independent linear systems , 1979, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Robson,et al.  Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field , 1981, Vision Research.

[21]  C. Johnson,et al.  Blue-on-yellow perimetry can predict the development of glaucomatous visual field loss. , 1993, Archives of ophthalmology.

[22]  J. Pokorny,et al.  The Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test in cone excitation space , 1993 .

[23]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Differential Adaptation of Cone Mechanisms Explains the Preferential Loss of Short-Wavelength Cone Sensitivity in Retinal Disease , 1989 .

[24]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Spectral sensitivity and wavelength discrimination of the human peripheral visual field. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[25]  Rhea T. Eskew,et al.  Peripheral chromatic sensitivity for flashes: A post-peceptoral red-green asymmetry , 1992, Vision Research.

[26]  Edward N. Pugh,et al.  Monochromatism determined at a long-wavelength/middle-wavelength cone-antagonistic locus , 1992, Vision Research.

[27]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Second-site adaptation in the red–green detection pathway: only elicited by low-spatial-frequency test stimuli , 1999, Vision Research.

[28]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Spectral sensitivity and adaptation characteristics of cone mechanisms under white-light adaptation. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[29]  E N Pugh,et al.  Red/Green color opponency at detection threshold. , 1983, Science.

[30]  K. Mullen,et al.  Postreceptoral chromatic detection mechanisms revealed by noise masking in three-dimensional cone contrast space. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[31]  M. Krystek,et al.  Systematics of human photopic spectral sensitivity , 1991 .

[32]  Louise L. Sloan The effect of intensity of light, state of adaptation of the eye, and size of photometric field on the visibility curve : a study of the purkinje phenomenon , 1928 .

[33]  V. Greenstein,et al.  Models of the normal and abnormal rod system , 1990, Vision Research.

[34]  E. Smith,et al.  Mechanisms mediating visual detection in static perimetry. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[35]  J. Pokorny,et al.  S-cone discrimination sensitivity and performance on arrangement tests , 1993 .

[36]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Receptive field structure in the primate retina , 1996, Vision Research.

[37]  J. Rovamo,et al.  Cortical magnification factor predicts the photopic contrast sensitivity of peripheral vision , 1978, Nature.

[38]  Qasim Zaidi,et al.  The effects of prolonged temporal modulation on the differential response of color mechanisms , 1992, Vision Research.

[39]  Paul R. Martin,et al.  Evidence that Blue‐on Cells are Part of the Third Geniculocortical Pathway in Primates , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Increment threshold and purity discrimination spectral sensitivities of X-chromosome-linked color-defective observers , 1996, Vision Research.

[41]  K. Mullen,et al.  Colour vision as a post-receptoral specialization of the central visual field , 1991, Vision Research.

[42]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  The design and use of a cone chromaticity space: A tutorial , 1996 .

[43]  S. L. Guth,et al.  Heterochromatic additivity, foveal spectral sensitivity, and a new color model. , 1973, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[44]  N. Bayley A study of fear by means of the psychogalvanic technique. , 1928 .

[45]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[46]  K. Mullen,et al.  Losses in Peripheral Colour Sensitivity Predicted from “Hit and Miss” Post-receptoral Cone Connections , 1996, Vision Research.

[47]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Chromatic discrimination with variation in chromaticity and luminance: Data and theory , 1993, Vision Research.

[48]  Robert M. Boynton,et al.  Chromatic difference steps of moderate size measured along theoretically critical axes , 1980 .

[49]  J. Odom,et al.  Distribution of carbonic anhydrase among human photoreceptors. , 1990, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[50]  M. Luckiesh,et al.  Color and vision. , 1915 .

[51]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Visual interactions with luminance and chromatic stimuli. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[52]  C. F. Stromeyer,et al.  Second-site adaptation in the red-green chromatic pathways , 1985, Vision Research.

[53]  Qasim Zaidi,et al.  The effect of adaptation on the differential sensitivity of the S-cone color system , 1992, Vision Research.

[54]  Vivianne C. Smith,et al.  Reaction times to chromatic stimuli , 1985, Vision Research.

[55]  R. F. Hess,et al.  The contrast sensitivity gradient across the human visual field: With emphasis on the low spatial frequency range , 1989, Vision Research.

[56]  A. Nagy,et al.  Red-green color discrimination as a function of stimulus field size in peripheral vision. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[57]  D. Foster,et al.  Test and field spectral sensitivities of colour mechanisms obtained on small white backgrounds: Actions of unitary opponent-colour processes? , 1983, Vision Research.

[58]  Thomas K. Kuyk,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the peripheral retina to large and small stimuli , 1982, Vision Research.

[59]  Barry B. Lee,et al.  The 'blue-on' opponent pathway in primate retina originates from a distinct bistratified ganglion cell type , 1994, Nature.

[60]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Human cones appear to adapt at low light levels: Measurements on the red—green detection mechanism , 1995, Vision Research.

[61]  V. Greenstein,et al.  The effects of light adaptation on L-cone sensitivity in retinal disease , 1992 .