Battle of the Mondrians: Investigating the Role of Unpredictability in Continuous Flash Suppression
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Grasping with the eyes: The role of elongation in visual recognition of manipulable objects , 2014, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[2] D. Alais,et al. The temporal frequency tuning of continuous flash suppression reveals peak suppression at very low frequencies , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[3] Philipp Sterzer,et al. Neural processing of visual information under interocular suppression: a critical review , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[4] W. Levelt,et al. The ‘laws’ of binocular rivalry: 50 years of Levelt’s propositions , 2015, Vision Research.
[5] Nir Levy,et al. Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[6] Eric Hiris,et al. The Informational Basis of Motion Coherence , 1995 .
[7] Sheng He,et al. Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression , 2007, Psychological science.
[8] Karl J. Friston. Am I Self-Conscious? (Or Does Self-Organization Entail Self-Consciousness?) , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[9] Sang Joon Kim,et al. A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .
[10] Richard H. A. H. Jacobs,et al. The time course of binocular rivalry reveals a fundamental role of noise. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[11] R. Blake,et al. On the use of continuous flash suppression for the study of visual processing outside of awareness , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[12] J. Enns,et al. What’s new in visual masking? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] P Vuilleumier,et al. Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[14] David Alais,et al. A bias for looming stimuli to predominate in binocular rivalry , 2007, Vision Research.
[15] U. Neisser,et al. Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] Randolph Blake,et al. Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’ , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[17] Chris L. E. Paffen,et al. Breaking continuous flash suppression: competing for consciousness on the pre-semantic battlefield , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[18] R. Blake,et al. Slow and steady, not fast and furious: Slow temporal modulation strengthens continuous flash suppression , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.
[19] David Alais,et al. Strength of continuous flash suppression is optimal when target and masker modulation rates are matched. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[20] J. Wagemans,et al. Moving Stimuli Are Less Effectively Masked Using Traditional Continuous Flash Suppression (CFS) Compared to a Moving Mondrian Mask (MMM): A Test Case for Feature-Selective Suppression and Retinotopic Adaptation , 2014, PloS one.
[21] S L Macknik,et al. The role of spatiotemporal edges in visibility and visual masking. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] Colin Blakemore,et al. Integration of motion information during binocular rivalry , 2002, Vision Research.
[23] R Blake,et al. Visual form created solely from temporal structure. , 1999, Science.
[24] C. Koch,et al. Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] David Alais,et al. Increasing depth of binocular rivalry suppression along two visual pathways , 2003, Vision Research.
[26] Miguel A. Vadillo,et al. Underpowered samples, false negatives, and unconscious learning , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] B. Sidis. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF SUGGESTION. , 1898, Science.
[28] R. Blake,et al. Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. , 2007, Emotion.
[29] D. Alais,et al. Orientation-tuned suppression in binocular rivalry reveals general and specific components of rivalry suppression. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[30] Randolph Blake,et al. Deconstructing continuous flash suppression. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[31] P. Sterzer,et al. Investigating Masked Priming Along the “Vision-for-Perception” and “Vision-for-Action” Dimensions of Unconscious Processing , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[32] Hiroki Yamamoto,et al. Activity in early visual areas predicts interindividual differences in binocular rivalry dynamics , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] Yusuke Murayama,et al. Attention But Not Awareness Modulates the BOLD Signal in the Human V1 During Binocular Suppression , 2011, Science.
[34] P. McGraw,et al. What determines the depth of interocular suppression during continuous flash suppression , 2013 .
[35] D. Heeger,et al. Continuous Flash Suppression Modulates Cortical Activity in Early Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] D. Alais. Binocular rivalry: competition and inhibition in visual perception. , 2012, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[37] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Smooth pursuit performance during target blanking does not influence the triggering of predictive saccades. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[38] J. Mattingley,et al. Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[39] Min-Suk Kang. Size matters: a study of binocular rivalry dynamics. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[40] Jerome D. Tietz,et al. Reaction time for horizontal versus vertical line-length discrimination , 1992 .
[41] B. Breitmeyer,et al. Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: A comparison, review, and update , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[42] Egor Ananyev,et al. Separate requirements for detection and perceptual stability of motion in interocular suppression , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[43] R. Blake,et al. The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages , 2005, Current Biology.
[44] F. Fang,et al. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[45] R. Blake,et al. Individual differences in continuous flash suppression: Potency and linkages to binocular rivalry dynamics , 2019, Vision Research.
[46] C. E. SHANNON,et al. A mathematical theory of communication , 1948, MOCO.
[47] Franco Lepore,et al. Spatial Frequency Tuning during the Conscious and Non-Conscious Perception of Emotional Facial Expressions – An Intracranial ERP Study , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[48] R. Blake. © 2001 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 5 A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies , 2000 .
[49] Jan Drewes,et al. The edge of awareness: Mask spatial density, but not color, determines optimal temporal frequency for continuous flash suppression. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[50] Randolph Blake,et al. Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[51] Dov Sagi,et al. Motion-induced blindness in normal observers , 2001, Nature.
[52] Bruno G. Breitmeyer,et al. Psychophysical “blinding” methods reveal a functional hierarchy of unconscious visual processing , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.
[53] D. Melcher,et al. Time for Awareness: The Influence of Temporal Properties of the Mask on Continuous Flash Suppression Effectiveness , 2016, PloS one.
[54] Philip H. Ramsey. Nonparametric Statistical Methods , 1974, Technometrics.
[55] Kevin C. Dieter,et al. When can attention influence binocular rivalry? , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[56] D. Alais,et al. Evidence for two interacting temporal channels in human visual processing , 2006, Vision Research.
[57] V. Franz,et al. Learning to detect but not to grasp suppressed visual stimuli , 2013, Neuropsychologia.
[58] Randolph Blake,et al. Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness , 2010, Current Biology.