Maternal stress and behavior modulate relationships between neonatal stress, attention, and basal cortisol at 8 months in preterm infants.

There is evidence that the developmental trajectory of cortisol secretion in preterm infants is altered, with elevated basal cortisol levels observed postnatally through at least 18 months corrected age (CA). This alteration is possibly due to neonatal pain-related stress. High cortisol levels might contribute to greater risk of impaired neurodevelopment. Since maternal factors are important for the regulation of infant stress responses, we investigated relationships between infant (neonatal pain-related stress, attention, cortisol) and maternal (stress, interactive behaviors) factors at age 8 months CA. We found that interactive maternal behaviors buffered the relationship between high neonatal pain-related stress exposure and poorer focused attention in mothers who self-reported low concurrent stress. Furthermore, in preterm infants exposed to high concurrent maternal stress and overwhelming interactive maternal behaviors, higher basal cortisol levels were associated with poor focused attention. Overall, these findings suggest that maternal factors can influence the cognitive resilience at 8 months of preterm infants exposed to early life stress.

[1]  K. Anand,et al.  Morphine Administration and Short-term Pulmonary Outcomes Among Ventilated Preterm Infants , 2005, Pediatrics.

[2]  D. Wiley,et al.  Perinatal Corticosteroids: A Review of Research Part I: Antenatal Administration , 2004, Neonatal Network.

[3]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Dampening of the cortisol response to handling at 3 months in human infants and its relation to sleep, circadian cortisol activity, and behavioral distress. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[4]  S. Matthews Antenatal glucocorticoids and the developing brain: mechanisms of action. , 2001, Seminars in neonatology : SN.

[5]  R. Grunau,et al.  Prior pain induces heightened motor responses during clustered care in preterm infants in the NICU. , 2005, Early human development.

[6]  E. Lieberman,et al.  Narrowing of Sex Differences in Infant Mortality in Massachusetts , 2004, Journal of Perinatology.

[7]  P. Wadhwa,et al.  The neurobiology of stress in human pregnancy: implications for prematurity and development of the fetal central nervous system. , 2001, Progress in brain research.

[8]  J. Buitelaar,et al.  Development of cortisol circadian rhythm in infancy. , 2003, Early human development.

[9]  M. Gunnar Reactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system to stressors in normal infants and children. , 1992, Pediatrics.

[10]  S. King,et al.  The effects of prenatal maternal stress on children's cognitive development: Project Ice Storm , 2005, Stress.

[11]  David W. Haley,et al.  Cortisol, contingency learning, and memory in preterm and full-term infants , 2006, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[12]  R. Grunau,et al.  Extremely premature (⩽ 800 g) schoolchildren: multiple areas of hidden disability , 1997, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[13]  M. Huffman,et al.  Maternal behavior and maternal stress are associated with infant behavioral development in macaques. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[14]  V. Glover Maternal stress or anxiety in pregnancy and emotional development of the child , 1997, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[15]  L. Doyle,et al.  Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[16]  T. Raivio,et al.  Circulating glucocorticoid bioactivity in the preterm newborn after antenatal betamethasone treatment. , 2004, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[17]  H. Ruff,et al.  Early Focused Attention Predicts Outcome for Children Born Prematurely , 2004, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[18]  J. Oehler,et al.  Developmental Outcome of Very Low Birth Weight Infants at Four Years of Age as a Function of Biological Risk and Psychosocial Risk , 1997, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[19]  G. Hindmarsh,et al.  Gender differences in cognitive abilities at 2 years in ELBW infants. Extremely low birth weight. , 2000, Early human development.

[20]  Vincent Nelson,et al.  Quantifying blood leakage into the oral mucosa and its effects on the measurement of cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone, and testosterone in saliva , 2004, Hormones and Behavior.

[21]  A. Moreira,et al.  The emergence of salivary cortisol circadian rhythm and its relationship to sleep activity in preterm infants , 2000, Clinical endocrinology.

[22]  C. Johnston,et al.  Repeated neonatal pain influences maternal behavior, but not stress responsiveness in rat offspring. , 2003, Brain research. Developmental brain research.

[23]  U. Nater,et al.  Psychological determinants of the cortisol stress response: the role of anticipatory cognitive appraisal , 2005, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[24]  M. Greenberg,et al.  Maternal stress and social support: effects on the mother-infant relationship from birth to eighteen months. , 1984, The American journal of orthopsychiatry.

[25]  B. McEwen,et al.  Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[26]  J. DiPietro,et al.  Maternal psychological distress during pregnancy in relation to child development at age two. , 2006, Child development.

[27]  A. Mirsky Disorders of attention: A neuropsychological perspective. , 1996 .

[28]  B. Fiese,et al.  Parent-infant interaction as a mediator of the relation between neonatal risk status and 12-month cognitive development , 2001 .

[29]  H. Ruff Components of attention during infants' manipulative exploration. , 1986, Child development.

[30]  Herbert G. Vaughan,et al.  Early identification of infants with developmental disabilities , 1988 .

[31]  K. Watterberg,et al.  Impaired Glucocorticoid Synthesis in Premature Infants Developing Chronic Lung Disease , 2001, Pediatric Research.

[32]  M. Greenberg,et al.  Longitudinal predictors of developmental status and social interaction in premature and full-term infants at age two. , 1988, Child development.

[33]  J W Mason,et al.  A Review of Psychoendocrine Research on the Sympathetic‐Adrenal Medullary System , 1968, Psychosomatic medicine.

[34]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Assessing salivary cortisol in studies of child development. , 1998, Child development.

[35]  S. Gross,et al.  Adrenal steroidogenesis in very low birth weight preterm infants. , 1994, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[36]  H. Eriksen,et al.  Psychobiological mechanisms of socioeconomic differences in health. , 2004, Social science & medicine.

[37]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[38]  R. R. Abidin Psychological Assessment Resources , 1995 .

[39]  M. Meaney,et al.  Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[40]  J. Smyth,et al.  STRESSORS AND MOOD MEASURED ON A MOMENTARY BASIS ARE ASSOCIATED WITH SALIVARY CORTISOL SECRETION , 1998, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[41]  T. Bredy,et al.  Partial reversal of the effect of maternal care on cognitive function through environmental enrichment , 2003, Neuroscience.

[42]  Maria Fitzgerald,et al.  The development of nociceptive circuits , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  R. R. Abidin Parenting Stress Index: Professional Manual . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources , 1995 .

[44]  David W. Haley,et al.  Neonatal procedural pain exposure predicts lower cortisol and behavioral reactivity in preterm infants in the NICU , 2005, Pain.

[45]  Marcie C. Goeke-Morey,et al.  Brief report: birth status, medical complications, and social environment: individual differences in development of preterm, very low birth weight infants. , 2000, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[46]  P. S. Zeskind,et al.  Biobehavioral Assessment of the Infant , 2001 .

[47]  P. Sauer,et al.  Adrenal Function in Sick Very Preterm Infants , 2000, Pediatric Research.

[48]  M. Meaney,et al.  Environmental Enrichment Reverses the Effects of Maternal Separation on Stress Reactivity , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Gunnar,et al.  The effects of morning naps, car trips, and maternal separation on adrenocortical activity in human infants. , 1991, Child development.

[50]  François Ansermet,et al.  Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child interactions. , 2004, Early human development.

[51]  J. Perlman Neurology of the newborn, 4th edition: By Joseph J. Volpe, 912 pp., illustrated. Philadelphia: WB Saunders Company, 2001, $105.00 ISBN 0-7216-8448-3. , 2001 .

[52]  A. Salvator,et al.  Maternal psychological distress and parenting stress after the birth of a very low-birth-weight infant. , 1999, JAMA.

[53]  S. Maccari,et al.  Antenatal glucocorticoids blunt the functioning of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of neonates and disturb some behaviors in juveniles , 2005, Neuroscience.

[54]  S. A. Rose,et al.  Processing speed in the 1st year of life: a longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[55]  H. Ruff,et al.  Long-term stability of individual differences in sustained attention in the early years. , 1990, Child development.

[56]  M Lerman,et al.  Biological and environmental correlates of developmental outcome of prematurely born infants in early adolescence. , 1994, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[57]  P. Zelazo,et al.  Stress During Pregnancy Affects General Intellectual and Language Functioning in Human Toddlers , 2004, Pediatric Research.

[58]  T. Fok,et al.  Transient adrenocortical insufficiency of prematurity and systemic hypotension in very low birthweight infants , 2004, Archives of Disease in Childhood - Fetal and Neonatal Edition.

[59]  J. Feldman,et al.  Pathways from prematurity and infant abilities to later cognition. , 2005, Child development.

[60]  B. McEwen,et al.  Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status , 2001, Development and Psychopathology.

[61]  M. Hofer Early relationships as regulators of infant physiology and behavior , 1994, Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992). Supplement.

[62]  J. Magill-Evans,et al.  Parent-Child Interactions and Development of Toddlers Born Preterm , 1999, Western journal of nursing research.

[63]  R. Grunau Early pain in preterm infants. A model of long-term effects. , 2002, Clinics in perinatology.

[64]  M. McCarthy,et al.  Sex Differences and Hormonal Effects in a Model of Preterm Infant Brain Injury , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[65]  B. Schone,et al.  Maternal psychological distress: the role of children's health. , 1996, Women & health.

[66]  T. Field,et al.  The effects of mother's physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[67]  C. Nemeroff,et al.  Long-Term Behavioral Effects of Repetitive Pain in Neonatal Rat Pups , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[68]  G. Lyon,et al.  Attention, Memory, and Executive Function , 1995 .

[69]  A. Fanaroff Morphine Administration and Short-term Pulmonary Outcomes Among Ventilated Preterm Infants , 2006 .

[70]  R. Grunau,et al.  Pattern of learning disabilities in children with extremely low birth weight and broadly average intelligence. , 2002, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[71]  M. Gunnar Quality of early care and buffering of neuroendocrine stress reactions: potential effects on the developing human brain. , 1998, Preventive medicine.

[72]  M. Posner,et al.  Images of mind , 1994 .

[73]  C. Nemeroff,et al.  Cortisol concentrations in 12- to 18-Month-Old infants: stability over time, location, and stressor , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[74]  N. Kalin,et al.  Maternal stress beginning in infancy may sensitize children to later stress exposure: effects on cortisol and behavior , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[75]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Rising cortisol at childcare: relations with nap, rest, and temperament. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[76]  G. Spangler,et al.  Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. , 1994, Developmental psychobiology.

[77]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations , 1996 .

[78]  David W. Haley,et al.  Altered basal cortisol levels at 3, 6, 8 and 18 months in infants born at extremely low gestational age. , 2007, The Journal of pediatrics.

[79]  R. J. Thompson,et al.  Developmental Outcome of Very Low Birth Weight Infants as a Function of Biological Risk and Psychosocial Risk , 1994, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[80]  R. Grunau,et al.  Psychosocial and Academic Characteristics of Extremely Low Birth Weight (≤800 g) Adolescents Who Are Free of Major Impairment Compared With Term-Born Control Subjects , 2004, Pediatrics.

[81]  S. Mangelsdorf,et al.  Attachment, Temperament, and Adrenocortical Activity in Infancy. A Study of Psychoendocrine Regulation. , 1989 .

[82]  S A Rose,et al.  Attention and recognition memory in the 1st year of life: a longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[83]  M. Greenberg,et al.  Effects of maternal age on parenting role. , 1982 .

[84]  L. Hellström-Westas,et al.  Preterm male infants need more initial respiratory and circulatory support than female infants , 2004, Acta paediatrica.

[85]  L. Rajagopalan,et al.  Serum adrenal steroid concentrations in premature infants. , 1989, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[86]  L. Vatten,et al.  Offspring sex and pregnancy outcome by length of gestation. , 2004, Early human development.

[87]  J. Volpe Neurology of the Newborn , 1959, Major problems in clinical pediatrics.

[88]  M. Austin,et al.  Maternal stress and obstetric and infant outcomes: epidemiological findings and neuroendocrine mechanisms , 2000, The Australian & New Zealand journal of obstetrics & gynaecology.

[89]  K. Anand,et al.  Effects of perinatal pain and stress. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[90]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development , 2002, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[91]  D. Streiner,et al.  School difficulties at adolescence in a regional cohort of children who were extremely low birth weight. , 2000, Pediatrics.

[92]  F. Duffy,et al.  Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant. Medical and neurofunctional effects. , 1994, JAMA.

[93]  R. Grunau,et al.  Neonatal procedural pain and preterm infant cortisol response to novelty at 8 months. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[94]  V. Glover,et al.  Glucocorticoid Exposure in Preterm Babies Predicts Saliva Cortisol Response to Immunization at 4 Months , 2005, Pediatric Research.

[95]  A. Bhutta,et al.  Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. , 2002, JAMA.

[96]  Social interaction and developmental competence of preterm and full-term infants during the first year of life. , 1983, Child development.

[97]  R. Haslam,et al.  Neonatal adrenal function after repeat dose prenatal corticosteroids: a randomized controlled trial. , 2006, American journal of obstetrics and gynecology.

[98]  T. Combs-Orme,et al.  Do maternal concerns at delivery predict parenting stress during infancy? , 2004, Child abuse & neglect.

[99]  D. Kurtzberg,et al.  Preterm infants' manipulative exploration of objects. , 1984, Child development.

[100]  Christopher Schatschneider,et al.  School-Age Outcomes in Children with Birth Weights under 750 g , 1994 .