Vibrotactile thresholds at the fingertip, volar forearm, large toe, and heel

Thresholds for the perception of vibration vary with location on the body due to the organization of tactile channels in hairy and non-hairy skin, and variations in receptor density. This study determined vibration thresholds at four locations on the body with two different contactors so as to assist the identification of the tactile channel determining the threshold at each location. Vibrotactile thresholds at six frequencies from 8 to 250 Hz were measured on the distal phalanx of the index finger, the volar forearm, the large toe, and the heel with two contactors: (i) a 1-mm diameter circular probe with a 1-mm gap to a fixed circular surround (i.e., 7.1-mm2 excitation area), and (ii) a 6-mm diameter circular probe with a 2-mm gap to a fixed circular surround (i.e., 79-mm2 excitation area). At all frequencies and with both contactors, thresholds on the fingertip were lower than thresholds on the volar forearm, the large toe, and the heel, consistent with a greater density of mechanoreceptors at the fingertip. Thresholds with the larger contactor were lower than thresholds with the smaller contactor on the fingertip at high frequencies (63, 125, and 250 Hz), on the large toe (except at 250 Hz), on the heel (at all frequencies), and on the volar forearm at 250 Hz. It is concluded that at least two tactile channels (Pacinian from 63 to 250 Hz, and non-Pacinian from 8 to 31.5 Hz) determined vibrotactile thresholds at the fingertip, whereas non-Pacinian channels had a dominant influence on vibrotactile thresholds at the volar forearm. The role of Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels could not be confirmed at the large toe or the heel despite some evidence of spatial summation.

[1]  J. T. Pilcher,et al.  Diseases of the Skin , 1903, The Indian Medical Gazette.

[2]  H. Montgomery,et al.  Diseases of the Skin , 1920, The Indian Medical Gazette.

[3]  Georg v. Békésy,et al.  A New Audiometer , 1947 .

[4]  E. Gregg Absolute measurement of the vibratory threshold. , 1951, A.M.A. archives of neurology and psychiatry.

[5]  A. Wilska On the vibrational sensitivity in different regions of the body surface. , 1954, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[6]  R. T. Verrillo,et al.  Investigation of Some Parameters of the Cutaneous Threshold for Vibration , 1962 .

[7]  R. T. Verrillo,et al.  Effect of Contactor Area on the Vibrotactile Threshold , 1963 .

[8]  R T Verrillo,et al.  Vibrotactile thresholds for hairy skin. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  R T Verrillo,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for a triplex system of cutaneous mechanoreception. , 1979, Sensory processes.

[10]  R. Johansson,et al.  Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  Heikki Hämäläinen,et al.  Peripheral neural basis of tactile sensations in man: II. Characteristics of human mechanoreceptors in the hairy skin and correlations of their activity with tactile sensations , 1981, Brain Research.

[12]  R. Johansson,et al.  Sensitivity to edges of mechanoreceptive afferent units innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand , 1982, Brain Research.

[13]  S J Bolanowski,et al.  Temperature and criterion effects in a somatosensory subsystem: a neurophysiological and psychophysical study. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  The effects of skin temperature on the psychophysical responses to vibration on glabrous and hairy skin. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  J. Abbs,et al.  Finger movement responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the dorsal skin of the human hand. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  M J Griffin,et al.  Factors influencing vibration sense thresholds used to assess occupational exposures to hand transmitted vibration. , 1991, British journal of industrial medicine.

[18]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Hairy skin: psychophysical channels and their physiological substrates. , 1994, Somatosensory & motor research.

[19]  A B Vallbo,et al.  Receptive field characteristics of tactile units with myelinated afferents in hairy skin of human subjects. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  U. Peiper,et al.  Two-point Discrimination of Vibratory Perception on the Sole of the Human Foot , 1996, Foot & ankle international.

[21]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  The Psychophysics of Tactile Perception and its Peripheral Physiological Basis , 1996 .

[22]  M. Abrahamowicz,et al.  Normal distributions of thermal and vibration sensory thresholds , 1998, Muscle & nerve.

[23]  B. Nigg,et al.  Quantifying a relationship between tactile and vibration sensitivity of the human foot with plantar pressure distributions during gait. , 1999, Clinical biomechanics.

[24]  G. T. Coleman,et al.  Transmission security for single, hair follicle-related tactile afferent fibers and their target cuneate neurons in cat. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Mats Trulsson,et al.  Mechanoreceptive afferents in the human sural nerve , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. T. Inglis,et al.  Distribution and behaviour of glabrous cutaneous receptors in the human foot sole , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Effect of aging on the subjective magnitude of vibration , 2002, Somatosensory & motor research.

[28]  Heikki Hämäläinen,et al.  Mechanical sensibility of the sole of the foot determined with vibratory stimuli of varying frequency , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  M. Rowe,et al.  Vibrotactile frequency discrimination in human hairy skin. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  M. Griffin,et al.  Effect of contact location on vibrotactile thresholds at the fingertip , 2006, Somatosensory & motor research.