Behavioral Responses to Epidemics in an Online Experiment: Using Virtual Diseases to Study Human Behavior

We report the results of a study we conducted using a simple multiplayer online game that simulates the spread of an infectious disease through a population composed of the players. We use our virtual epidemics game to examine how people respond to epidemics. The analysis shows that people's behavior is responsive to the cost of self-protection, the reported prevalence of disease, and their experiences earlier in the epidemic. Specifically, decreasing the cost of self-protection increases the rate of safe behavior. Higher reported prevalence also raises the likelihood that individuals would engage in self-protection, where the magnitude of this effect depends on how much time has elapsed in the epidemic. Individuals' experiences in terms of how often an infection was acquired when they did not engage in self-protection are another factor that determines whether they will invest in preventive measures later on. All else being equal, individuals who were infected at a higher rate are more likely to engage in self-protective behavior compared to those with a lower rate of infection. Lastly, fixing everything else, people's willingness to engage in safe behavior waxes or wanes over time, depending on the severity of an epidemic: when prevalence is high, people are more likely to adopt self-protective measures as time goes by; when prevalence is low, a ‘self-protection fatigue’ effect sets in whereby individuals are less willing to engage in safe behavior over time.

[1]  Joel E. Cohen,et al.  Infectious Diseases of Humans: Dynamics and Control , 1992 .

[2]  John J. Grefenstette,et al.  A game dynamic model for vaccine skeptics and vaccine believers: measles as an example. , 2012, Journal of theoretical biology.

[3]  D. Earn,et al.  Vaccination and the theory of games. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Eli P. Fenichel,et al.  Adaptive human behavior in epidemiological models , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  HIV Prevention Needs and Successes: a tale of three countries An update on HIV prevention success in Senegal, Thailand and Uganda , 2001 .

[6]  Robert J. Smith,et al.  Halting HIV/AIDS with avatars and havatars: a virtual world approach to modelling epidemics , 2009, BMC public health.

[7]  Alison P Galvani,et al.  The dynamics of risk perceptions and precautionary behavior in response to 2009 (H1N1) pandemic influenza , 2010, BMC infectious diseases.

[8]  J. Heckman,et al.  Lab Experiments Are a Major Source of Knowledge in the Social Sciences , 2009, Science.

[9]  Frederick Chen,et al.  Rational behavioral response and the transmission of STDs. , 2004, Theoretical population biology.

[10]  Gerjo Kok,et al.  Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: results of three consecutive online surveys , 2011, BMC public health.

[11]  Frederick Chen A Susceptible-infected Epidemic Model with Voluntary Vaccinations , 2006, Journal of mathematical biology.

[12]  J. Kagel,et al.  Handbook of Experimental Economics , 1997 .

[13]  C T Bauch,et al.  The impact of personal experiences with infection and vaccination on behaviour-incidence dynamics of seasonal influenza. , 2012, Epidemics.

[14]  K. Choi,et al.  Avoidance behaviors and negative psychological responses in the general population in the initial stage of the H1N1 pandemic in Hong Kong , 2010, BMC infectious diseases.

[15]  P. Kaye Infectious diseases of humans: Dynamics and control , 1993 .

[16]  G. Rubin,et al.  Health Protection Agency, , 2011 .

[17]  Rachel T. A. Croson,et al.  Gender Differences in Preferences , 2009 .

[18]  Carl T. Bergstrom,et al.  A public choice framework for controlling transmissible and evolving diseases , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  S. Blower,et al.  Mean-field analysis of an inductive reasoning game: application to influenza vaccination. , 2007, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[20]  C. Plott,et al.  Handbook of Experimental Economics Results , 2008 .

[21]  T. Hallett,et al.  Declines in HIV prevalence can be associated with changing sexual behaviour in Uganda, urban Kenya, Zimbabwe, and urban Haiti , 2006, Sexually Transmitted Infections.

[22]  J. Lau,et al.  Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: from day 10 to day 62 , 2003, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[23]  Constance A Nyamukapa,et al.  HIV Decline Associated with Behavior Change in Eastern Zimbabwe , 2006, Science.

[24]  G. Chapman,et al.  Predictors of influenza vaccine acceptance among healthy adults. , 1999, Preventive medicine.

[25]  Timothy C. Reluga,et al.  Evolving public perceptions and stability in vaccine uptake. , 2006, Mathematical biosciences.

[26]  J. Buehler,et al.  Condom Use and Risk of Gonorrhea and Chlamydia: A Systematic Review of Design and Measurement Factors Assessed in Epidemiologic Studies , 2006, Sexually transmitted diseases.

[27]  K. Choi,et al.  Prevalence of preventive behaviors and associated factors during early phase of the H1N1 influenza epidemic , 2010, American Journal of Infection Control.

[28]  Daniel Low-Beer,et al.  Population-Level HIV Declines and Behavioral Risk Avoidance in Uganda , 2004, Science.