Disruption of the prefrontal cortex function by rTMS produces a category-specific enhancement of the reaction times during visual object identification

Object identification is enabled through a distributed neural network but the relative contribution of the single components of this network is largely unknown. In the present study, we used online interference by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to investigate the role of the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in identifying semantically different stimuli presented as to make the decision process easy or difficult, according to the amount of sensory information available. Nineteen healthy volunteers performed an object identification task. Stimuli belonging to living and non-living categories were presented at different levels of spatial filtering following a coarse-to-fine order that gradually integrated spatial information. Six-pulse trains of 10-Hz rTMS were delivered at an intensity of 90% resting motor threshold simultaneously to the picture presentation. rTMS of either the left or right DLPFC produced a significant lengthening in the identification process of spatially filtered living stimuli, as shown by the increase in the reaction time, but not of non-filtered living stimuli or of non-living objects. rTMS over the vertex did not interfere with the identification task. These data indicate that DLPFC role in the network underlying object recognition is more crucial when this neural process is challenged by the level of sensory information available to the observer. Specificity of this effect for living objects is discussed taking into account the crucial role of DLPFC in recruitment of cognitive resources for accomplishing perceptual decision-making.

[1]  M. Vannucci,et al.  Identification of spatially filtered stimuli as function of the semantic category. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  R. Malach,et al.  The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  B L Day,et al.  Delay in the execution of voluntary movement by electrical or magnetic brain stimulation in intact man. Evidence for the storage of motor programs in the brain. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Interference with Performance of a Response Selection Task that has no Working Memory Component: An rTMS Comparison of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Medial Frontal Cortex , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  G. Deco,et al.  A hierarchical neural system with attentional top–down enhancement of the spatial resolution for object recognition , 2000, Vision Research.

[6]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  The organization of conceptual knowledge: the evidence from category-specific semantic deficits , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  J. Leon-Carrion,et al.  A lasting post-stimulus activation on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is produced when processing valence and arousal in visual affective stimuli , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[8]  C Frith,et al.  Brain mechanisms associated with top-down processes in perception. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .

[10]  Manila Vannucci,et al.  A New Standardized Set of Ecological Pictures for Experimental and Clinical Research on Visual Object Processing , 2004, Cortex.

[11]  J R Lishman,et al.  Role of coarse and fine spatial information in face and object processing. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[13]  Susan M. Ravizza,et al.  Contributions of the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia to Set Shifting , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Dynamics of visual recognition revealed by fMRI , 2006, NeuroImage.

[15]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience – virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  G. V. Dearborn,et al.  PROFESSOR MOSSO'S LECTURES AT THE CLARK UNIVERSITY DECENNIAL , 1900 .

[17]  M. Viggiano,et al.  Category-specific visual recognition as affected by aging and expertise. , 2006, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[18]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[20]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Human efficiency for recognizing and detecting low-pass filtered objects , 1995, Vision Research.

[21]  P M Rossini,et al.  The role of the left frontal lobe in action naming: rTMS evidence , 2002, Neurology.

[22]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Neuroimaging evidence for object model verification theory: Role of prefrontal control in visual object categorization , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Viggiano,et al.  Recognition of category-related visual stimuli in Parkinson's disease: Before and after pharmacological treatment , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[25]  M Gangitano,et al.  Segregation of areas related to visual working memory in the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Ulf Ziemann,et al.  Delay in simple reaction time after focal transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human brain occurs at the final motor output stage , 1997, Brain Research.

[27]  Manila Vannucci,et al.  Category Effects on the Processing of Plane-Rotated Objects , 2000, Perception.

[28]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  A. Damasio,et al.  A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M C Morrone,et al.  Recognition of Positive and Negative Bandpass-Filtered Images , 1986, Perception.

[31]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[33]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[35]  S. Grossberg How does a brain build a cognitive code , 1980 .

[36]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  H. Hughes,et al.  Global Precedence, Spatial Frequency Channels, and the Statistics of Natural Images , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  Bart Rypma,et al.  Prefrontal modulation of working memory performance in brain injury and disease , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[40]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Semantic Priming in Parkinsons Disease: Evidence for Delayed Spreading Activation , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[41]  B. Mesquita,et al.  Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .

[42]  M. Bar A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Petrides Dissociable Roles of Mid-Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex in Visual Working Memory , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  M. Kutas,et al.  The covert interplay between perception and memory: event-related potential evidence. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[45]  Erik Stalberg,et al.  Pre-movement facilitation of motor-evoked potentials in man during transcranial stimulation of the central motor pathways , 1988, Brain Research.

[46]  M. Rushworth,et al.  A primer of magnetic stimulation as a tool for neuropsychology. , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  A. Korczyn Cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease , 2003 .

[48]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[49]  G. Zaccara,et al.  Category-specific visual identification of filtered objects in Alzheimer's disease. , 2007, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[50]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Summation of semantic priming and complex sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[51]  Carlo Miniussi,et al.  Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on action naming in patients with Alzheimer disease. , 2006, Archives of neurology.

[52]  M. Kutas,et al.  Overt and covert identification of fragmented objects inferred from performance and electrophysiological measures. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  M. Bar,et al.  Cortical Mechanisms Specific to Explicit Visual Object Recognition , 2001, Neuron.

[54]  M. Bar Visual objects in context , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  S. Kéri The cognitive neuroscience of category learning , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[56]  Claudio Babiloni,et al.  Prefontal cortex in long-term memory: an “interference” approach using magnetic stimulation , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Premotor and Prefrontal Correlates of Category-Related Lexical Retrieval , 1998, NeuroImage.

[58]  Stephen A. Engel,et al.  Linearity across spatial frequency in object recognition , 1998, Vision Research.

[59]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[60]  Manila Vannucci,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry for spatially filtered stimuli belonging to different semantic categories. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[61]  Stephan Felber,et al.  The neural regions sustaining episodic encoding and recognition of objects , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[62]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.