The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision.

A compelling single case report of visual awareness (visual qualia) without primary visual cortex would be sufficient to refute the hypothesis that the primary visual cortex and the back-projections to it are necessary for conscious visual experience. In a previous study, we emphasized the presence of crude visual awareness in Patient G.Y., with a lesion of the primary visual cortex, who is aware of, and able to discriminate, fast-moving visual stimuli presented to his blind field. The visual nature of Patient G.Y.'s blind field experience has since been questioned and it has been suggested that the special circumstances of repeated testing over decades may have altered Patient G.Y.'s visual pathways. We therefore sought new evidence of visual awareness without primary visual cortex in patients for whom such considerations do not apply. Three patients with hemianopic field defects (Patient G.N. and Patient F.B. with MRI confirmed primary visual cortex lesions, Patient C.G. with an inferred lesion) underwent detailed psychophysical testing in their blind fields. Visual stimuli were presented at different velocities and contrasts in two- and four-direction discrimination experiments and the direction of motion and awareness reported using a forced-choice paradigm. Detailed verbal reports were also obtained of the nature of the blind field experience with comparison of the drawings of the stimulus presented in the blind and intact fields, where possible. All three patients reported visual awareness in their blind fields. Visual awareness was significantly more likely when a moving stimulus was present compared to no stimulus catch trials (P < 0.01 for each subject). Psychophysical performance in Patient F.B. and Patient G.N. was consistent with the Riddoch syndrome, with higher levels of visual awareness for moving compared to static stimuli (P < 0.001) and intact direction discrimination (P < 0.0001 for two- and four-direction experiments). Although the blind field experience of all three subjects was degraded, it was clearly visual in nature. We conclude that the primary visual cortex or back-projections to it are not necessary for visual awareness.

[1]  Saâd Jbabdi,et al.  Changes in connectivity after visual cortical brain damage underlie altered visual function. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  Roelfsema Pieter Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping , 2008 .

[3]  S. Zeki,et al.  The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V1 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex , 1989, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al.  Unconscious vision: new insights into the neuronal correlate of blindsight using diffusion tractography. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Alwyn C. Scott,et al.  Towards a Science of Consciousness , 1996 .

[6]  S. Zeki,et al.  The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  G. Riddoch DISSOCIATION OF VISUAL PERCEPTIONS DUE TO OCCIPITAL INJURIES, WITH ESPECIAL REFERENCE TO APPRECIATION OF MOVEMENT , 1917 .

[8]  S. Shipp,et al.  The functional logic of cortical connections , 1988, Nature.

[9]  R. Werth,et al.  Visual functions without the occipital lobe or after cerebral hemispherectomy in infancy , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nucleus , 2010, Nature.

[11]  G. Holmes DISTURBANCES OF VISION BY CEREBRAL LESIONS , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[12]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Residual vision in a scotoma: implications for blindsight. , 1992, Science.

[13]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  A re-evaluation of blindsight and the role of striate cortex (V1) in visual awareness , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  E. Warrington,et al.  "Blindsight": Vision in a field defect. , 1974, Lancet.

[17]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  A. Cowey The blindsight saga , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  A. Cowey,et al.  Motion discrimination in cortically blind patients. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  Double dissociation of V1 and V5/MT activity in visual awareness. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  R. Goebel,et al.  Is V1 Necessary for Conscious Vision in Areas of Relative Cortical Blindness? , 2001, NeuroImage.

[22]  C. Koch,et al.  A framework for consciousness , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  L. Benevento,et al.  The organization of connections between the pulvinar and visual area MT in the macaque monkey , 1983, Brain Research.

[24]  Frank Tong,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Primary visual cortex and visual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  Alan Cowey,et al.  The 30th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: Fact, Artefact, and Myth about Blindsight , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  A. Cowey,et al.  Striate cortex (V1) activity gates awareness of motion , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Cowey,et al.  Magnetically induced phosphenes in sighted, blind and blindsighted observers , 2000, Neuroreport.

[28]  Lawrence C. Sincich,et al.  Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  T Landis,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for fast visual processing through the human koniocellular pathway when stimuli move. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  James Danckert,et al.  Blindsight in action: what can the different sub-types of blindsight tell us about the control of visually guided actions? , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  C. N. Guy,et al.  Motion specific responses from a blind hemifield. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  S. Zeki,et al.  A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Seeing without Seeing? Degraded Conscious Vision in a Blindsight Patient , 2008, PloS one.

[34]  B. Cragg The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the Nauta method. , 1969, Vision research.

[35]  H. Lau,et al.  Direct assessment of qualia in a blindsight participant , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[36]  S. Zeki,et al.  The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  L. Weiskrantz Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .

[38]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[39]  David H. Brainard,et al.  Neural Activity within Area V1 Reflects Unconscious Visual Performance in a Case of Blindsight , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Lawrence Weiskrantz,et al.  Consciousness and Commentaries , 1998 .

[41]  A. Morland,et al.  Visual perception of motion, luminance and colour in a human hemianope. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  E. Barth,et al.  Low-Level Phenomenal Vision Despite Unilateral Destruction of Primary Visual Cortex , 2001, Consciousness and Cognition.

[43]  Victor A. F. Lamme Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[44]  C. N. Guy,et al.  The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  M. Yukie,et al.  Direct projection from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex in macaque monkeys , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[46]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Perceptual Relearning of Complex Visual Motion after V1 Damage in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Jared Rutter,et al.  Regulation of Clock and NPAS2 DNA Binding by the Redox State of NAD Cofactors , 2001, Science.