The Network Architecture of Cortical Processing in Visuo-spatial Reasoning

Reasoning processes have been closely associated with prefrontal cortex (PFC), but specifically emerge from interactions among networks of brain regions. Yet it remains a challenge to integrate these brain-wide interactions in identifying the flow of processing emerging from sensory brain regions to abstract processing regions, particularly within PFC. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were collected while participants performed a visuo-spatial reasoning task. We found increasing involvement of occipital and parietal regions together with caudal-rostral recruitment of PFC as stimulus dimensions increased. Brain-wide connectivity analysis revealed that interactions between primary visual and parietal regions predominantly influenced activity in frontal lobes. Caudal-to-rostral influences were found within left-PFC. Right-PFC showed evidence of rostral-to-caudal connectivity in addition to relatively independent influences from occipito-parietal cortices. In the context of hierarchical views of PFC organization, our results suggest that a caudal-to-rostral flow of processing may emerge within PFC in reasoning tasks with minimal top-down deductive requirements.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modeling , 2010, Scholarpedia.

[2]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[4]  R. Snow,et al.  Lecture 2: Toward a Theory of Cognitive Aptitude for Learning from Instruction , 1984 .

[5]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  THE CONTROL OF THE FALSE DISCOVERY RATE IN MULTIPLE TESTING UNDER DEPENDENCY , 2001 .

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[7]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Prefrontal Dynamics Underlying Rapid Instructed Task Learning Reverse with Practice , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Desmond,et al.  Neural Substrates of Fluid Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Neocortical Activation during Performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  J. D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Anil K. Seth,et al.  A MATLAB toolbox for Granger causal connectivity analysis , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[11]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Mapping directed influence over the brain using Granger causality and fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[12]  J. Pearl Causality: Models, Reasoning and Inference , 2000 .

[13]  Donald J. Reichard,et al.  A SELECTIVE REVIEW , 2007 .

[14]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[16]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning , 2001, NeuroImage.

[17]  R. Fisher FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTION OF THE VALUES OF THE CORRELATION COEFFIENTS IN SAMPLES FROM AN INDEFINITELY LARGE POPU;ATION , 1915 .

[18]  G. Schwarz Estimating the Dimension of a Model , 1978 .

[19]  K. C. Anderson,et al.  Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules , 2001, Nature.

[20]  T. Paus Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: A selective review , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval , 1999, Nature.

[22]  J. Geweke,et al.  Measures of Conditional Linear Dependence and Feedback between Time Series , 1984 .

[23]  J. Ashburner,et al.  Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[24]  David J. Freedman,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: categories, concepts and cognition. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  C. Granger Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods , 1969 .

[26]  J. Fuster The cognit: a network model of cortical representation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[27]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[28]  Gian Luca Romani,et al.  Differential patterns of cortical activation as a function of fluid reasoning complexity , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[29]  J. Duncan,et al.  Lateral Prefrontal Cortex Subregions Make Dissociable Contributions during Fluid Reasoning , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  D. Pandya,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  M Petrides,et al.  Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[35]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Searching for “the Top” in Top-Down Control , 2005, Neuron.