Taking threats to the lab: Introducing an experimental paradigm for studying verbal threats.

People who threaten to cause harm may either actualize their threat or bluff. To manage the risk that harmful acts will be perpetrated, it is of great importance to recognize differences between threatening behavior that will and will not be actualized. In this article we present what is, to our knowledge, the first study in which verbal threats are examined experimentally. We theorized that threats reflecting actual intentions come with implementation details (how one will actualize the threat), whereas bluffs linger in the formation of ideas (reasons why one makes a threat). In a mock-paradigm, participants (N = 181) threatened a company over the phone and were questioned about their threat during the call. Participants were either instructed not to actualize the threat (bluffers), to actualize it only if the company would not meet their demands (conditional actualizers) or to always actualize the threat (decisive actualizers). It was found that bluffers and actualizers differed in the amount of implementation details they provided. In contrast to our prediction, bluffers provided comparatively more details on implementation. Possible explanations for this result are discussed.

[1]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal-level theory of psychological distance. , 2010, Psychological review.

[2]  Thomas L. Webb,et al.  Implementation intentions and health behaviour , 2005 .

[3]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Accuracy of Deception Judgments , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[4]  Timothy J. Luke,et al.  How Awareness of Possible Evidence Induces Forthcoming Counter‐Interrogation Strategies , 2014 .

[5]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. , 1987 .

[6]  Mary Ellen O'Toole,et al.  The School Shooter: A Threat Assessment Perspective , 2000 .

[7]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[8]  P. Granhag,et al.  A new theoretical perspective on deception detection: On the psychology of instrumental mind-reading , 2008 .

[9]  Paul E. Mullen,et al.  The Psychological Basis of Threatening Behaviour , 2013 .

[10]  F. Farnham,et al.  The role of mental disorder in attacks on European politicians 1990–2004 , 2007, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[11]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes , 2006 .

[12]  R. Borum,et al.  Threat assessment: defining an approach for evaluating risk of targeted violence. , 1999, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[13]  Alison Ledgerwood,et al.  The effects of psychological distance on abstraction: Two meta-analyses. , 2015, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Paul E Mullen,et al.  A clinical study of those who utter threats to kill. , 2011, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[15]  Leif A. Strömwall,et al.  Discriminating between true and false intent among small cells of suspects , 2016 .

[16]  B. Vossekuil,et al.  Threat Assessment: An Approach To Prevent Targeted Violence: (517592006-001) , 1995 .

[17]  Aldert Vrij,et al.  A cognitive load approach to lie detection , 2008 .

[18]  Daniel A. Martell,et al.  Threatening and Otherwise Inappropriate Letters to Hollywood Celebrities , 1991 .

[19]  Thomas W. Milburn,et al.  On the nature of threat : a social psychological analysis , 1981 .

[20]  R. Purcell,et al.  Assessing and managing the risks in the stalking situation. , 2006, The journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law.

[21]  Maria Hartwig,et al.  Guilty and innocent suspects’ strategies during police interrogations , 2007 .

[22]  F. Farnham,et al.  THE FIXATED THREAT ASSESSMENT CENTRE – IMPLEMENTING A JOINT POLICING AND PSYCHIATRIC APPROACH TO RISK ASSESSMENT AND MANAGEMENT IN PUBLIC FIGURE THREAT CASES , 2014 .

[23]  D. Mook,et al.  In defense of external invalidity. , 1983 .

[24]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal Levels and Psychological Distance: Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation, and Behavior. , 2007, Journal of consumer psychology : the official journal of the Society for Consumer Psychology.

[25]  P. Granhag,et al.  Preventing Future Crimes Identifying Markers of True and False Intent , 2014 .

[26]  A. Vrij,et al.  Discriminating between True and False Intentions , 2014 .

[27]  David V James,et al.  Attacks on the British Royal family: the role of psychotic illness. , 2008, The journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law.

[28]  Leif A. Strömwall,et al.  Interviewing strategically to elicit admissions from guilty suspects. , 2015, Law and human behavior.

[29]  Y. Trope,et al.  Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: probability and the mental representation of events. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[31]  Stephen C. P. Wong,et al.  The efficacy of violence prediction: a meta-analytic comparison of nine risk assessment tools. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  P. Granhag,et al.  The Strategic Use of Evidence Technique , 2014 .

[33]  Pär Anders Granhag,et al.  Markers of good planning behavior as a cue for separating true and false intent. , 2013, PsyCh journal.

[34]  J. Meloy,et al.  Warning behaviours and their configurations accross various Domains of targeted violence , 2014 .

[35]  J R Meloy,et al.  Offender and offense characteristics of a nonrandom sample of mass murderers. , 1999, The journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law.

[36]  Don A. Andrews,et al.  The Risk-Need-Responsivity (RNR) Model , 2011 .

[37]  J Reid Meloy,et al.  The concept of leakage in threat assessment. , 2011, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[38]  P. Granhag,et al.  The Scharff-technique: eliciting intelligence from human sources. , 2014, Law and human behavior.