Pessimism-optimism and risk taking in individual and group contexts.

Choice-dilemmas items were administered with the customary risk-taking format (minimal acceptable odds to warrant recommendation of the desirable risky alternative) and with a pessimism-opti mism format (estimation of the actual real-world probability of success of the desirable risky alternative). Following individual testing to establish base-line levels, subjects were randomly assigned to either a risk-pessimism or pessimism-risk treatment. In the former, discussion to consensus was focused on the risk dimension, and subsequent pessimism-optimism judgments were made privately; in the latter, discussion to consensus was focused on the pessimism-optimism dimension, and subsequent risk decisions were made privately. Results indicated that the link between risk and pessimism was asymmetrical. The risky shift was produced only when discussion was oriented along the risk-taking dimension. In contrast, pessimistic shifts were obtained following both risk-oriented and pessimism-oriented discussions. Group-induced pessimism was attributed to pooling or heightened realization of the "things that might go wrong" in pursuing a risky course of action. As studies on the risky-shift phenomenon in groups continue to proliferate, investigators have begun to inquire into the conditions that militate against the phenomenon in both laboratory and real-world contexts. Thus, Madaras and Bern (1968) have recently pointed to three factors that place limits on the generalizability of risky-shift effects following group interaction: (a) moral or other "cost" considerations associated with the risky alternative; (b) the impingement of the decision consequences on persons other than those involved in the act of decision making; and (c) the observation that the shift toward risk is accompanied by a shift toward pessimism, the latter possibly having a decisive effect in a complex decision-making situation,

[1]  N. Kogan,et al.  Level of Risk Selected by Individuals and Groups When Deciding for Self and for Others , 1971 .

[2]  D. G. Pruitt,et al.  A reexamination of the familiarization hypothesis in group risk taking , 1970 .

[3]  Helmut Lamm,et al.  Risk taking in the context of intergroup negotiation , 1970 .

[4]  S. Moscovici,et al.  The group as a polarizer of attitudes. , 1969 .

[5]  W. Doise Intergroup relations and polarization of individual and collective judgments. , 1969 .

[6]  N. Kogan,et al.  LEVEL OF RISK SELECTED BY INDIVIDUALS AND GROUPS WHEN DECIDING FOR SELF AND FOR OTHERS1 , 1969 .

[7]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  Group risk-taking in a two-choice situation: Replication, extension, and a model☆ , 1969 .

[8]  Dean G. Pruitt,et al.  THE RISKY SHIFT IN GROUP BETTING. , 1969 .

[9]  D. J. Schneider,et al.  Test of the "risk is a value" hypothesis. , 1969 .

[10]  W. Doise,et al.  Effects on anticipated delegate status on level of risk taking in small decision-making groups , 1969 .

[11]  H. Alker,et al.  Effects of Norm-oriented Group Discussion on Individual Verbal Risk Taking and Conservatism , 1968 .

[12]  B. D. Jamieson The “Risky-Shift” Phenomenon with a Heterogeneous Sample , 1968, Psychological reports.

[13]  D. Bem,et al.  Risk and conservatism in group decision-making ☆ , 1968 .

[14]  G. Mandler,et al.  New Directions in Psychology III , 1968 .

[15]  M. Wallach,et al.  Is risk a value? , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  M. Wallach,et al.  ARE RISK TAKERS MORE PERSUASIVE THAN CONSERVATIVES IN GROUP DISCUSSION. , 1968 .

[17]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  Individual and group risk-taking in a two-choice situation , 1968 .

[18]  D. G. Pruitt,et al.  Components of group risk taking , 1967 .

[19]  D. Thistlethwaite,et al.  Effects of familiarization and group discussion upon risk taking. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  M. Wallach,et al.  Modification of a judgmental style through group interaction. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  N. Bateson Familiarization, group discussion, and risk taking , 1966 .

[22]  David L. Bradford,et al.  The Role of Social Norms and Leadership in Risk-Taking , 1966 .

[23]  M. Wallach,et al.  Risk Taking: A Study in Cognition and Personality , 1965 .

[24]  M. Wallach,et al.  The roles of information, discussion, and consensus in group risk taking , 1965 .

[25]  L. R. Hoffman Group Problem Solving1 , 1965 .

[26]  H. Guetzkow,et al.  A social psychology of group processes for decision-making , 1964 .

[27]  Y. Rim Social Attitudes and Risk-taking , 1964 .

[28]  D. Bem,et al.  DIFFUSION OF RESPONSIBILITY AND LEVEL OF RISK TAKING IN GROUPS. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  A. Zander,et al.  Individual and Group Levels of Aspiration , 1963 .

[30]  D. Bem,et al.  Group influence on individual risk taking. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[31]  M. Wallach,et al.  Aspects of judgment and decision making: interrelationships and changes with age. , 2007, Behavioral science.

[32]  M. Wallach,et al.  Sex differences and judgment processes1 , 1959 .

[33]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[34]  Vaughn J. Crandall,et al.  Expectancy Statements and Decision Times as Functions of Objective Probabilities and Reinforcement Values , 1955 .

[35]  F. W. Irwin,et al.  Stated expectations as functions of probability and desirability of outcomes. , 1953, Journal of personality.

[36]  Robert L. Thorndike,et al.  The Effect of Discussion upon the Correctness of Group Decisions, when the Factor of Majority Influence is Allowed For , 1938 .

[37]  M. E. Shaw A comparison of individuals and small groups in the rational solution of complex problems. , 1932 .