Two-component models of reaching: Evidence from deafferentation in a Fitts’ law task

Both feedforward and feedback mechanisms are used to ensure accurate movements. Feedback information comes primarily from vision and proprioception; the relative contributions of these modalities to on-line control of action and internal model maintenance remain unclear. We report data from an experiment in which a chronically deafferented subject (JDY) and nine controls were asked to reach to targets of different sizes both with and without vision. Movement times of controls were consistent with Fitts' law on trials with and without vision. JDY's movement times were consistent with Fitts' law only with vision. She was inaccurate relative to controls with vision but exhibited a significantly greater decrement in performance than controls without vision. Finally, JDY's performance on trials with vision deteriorated as a function of the number of preceding trials on which vision was not available. These data provide support for classical models of motor control that divide reaching into an initial ballistic movement guided by efference copy, and a terminal stage where sensory feedback is crucial. Furthermore, these data also demonstrate that proprioception is needed to calibrate and maintain internal models of action.

[1]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  A. H. Norris,et al.  Speed and accuracy of movement and their changes with age. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[4]  I. Scott MacKenzie,et al.  Fitts' Law as a Research and Design Tool in Human-Computer Interaction , 1992, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[5]  Janice Kay,et al.  An evaluation of statistical procedures for comparing an individual's performance with that of a group of controls , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[6]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. , 2006, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[7]  Les G. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981 .

[8]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Paul H. Garthwaite,et al.  Statistical Methods for Single-Case Studies in Neuropsychology: Comparing the Slope of a Patient's Regression Line with those of a Control Sample , 2004, Cortex.

[10]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[11]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[13]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Randomizing visual feedback in manual aiming: reminiscence of the previous trial condition and prior knowledge of feedback availability , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Derick T Wade,et al.  The Rivermead Assessment of Somatosensory Performance (RASP): standardization and reliability data , 2002, Clinical rehabilitation.

[15]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[16]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  Atsuo Murata,et al.  Extending Effective Target Width in Fitts' Law to a Two-Dimensional Pointing Task , 1999, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[19]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Accurate Reaching after Active But Not Passive Movements of the Hand: Evidence for Forward Modeling , 2008, Behavioural neurology.