Changes in grasping kinematics due to different start postures of the hand.

It was proposed that grasping is a relatively stereotyped movement pattern which can be subdivided into the components of manipulation, transport, and orientation of the hand. However, it is still a matter of debate whether these components are independent of each other. In three experiments we altered the start posture of the hand by either changing the size of the start aperture or the orientation of the hand prior to movement onset. The variation of the aperture size primarily affected the manipulation component of the grip resulting in an overall change of the pre-shaping profile. In contrast, an alteration of the start orientation affected the manipulation and the transport components to a similar extent. These results give further evidence that hand orientation is neither planned nor controlled independently from the other movement components. Moreover, when the grip had to match specific object properties, adjustments were mainly achieved within the first movement part. In contrast, when there were no movement constraints the final finger positions were influenced by the initial start posture of the hand. We found no evidence for a fixed spatial or temporal coupling of the grasp and the transport component in our experiments.

[1]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[2]  Reinoud J. Bootsma,et al.  Spatio-temporal organisation of natural prehension , 1992 .

[3]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Alterations in transport path differentially affect temporal and spatial movement parameters , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  A. Roby-Brami,et al.  Coupling between reaching movement direction and hand orientation for grasping , 2002, Brain Research.

[5]  Volker H Franz,et al.  Corrective Processes in Grasping After Perturbations of Object Size , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  U. Castiello,et al.  Reorganization of prehension components following perturbation of object size. , 1995, Psychology and aging.

[7]  M. Jeannerod Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects , 1981 .

[8]  P. Baraduc,et al.  Hand orientation for grasping depends on the direction of the reaching movement , 2000, Brain Research.

[9]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  U. Castiello,et al.  Reach to grasp: the response to a simultaneous perturbation of object position and size , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Eli Brenner,et al.  On the relation between object shape and grasping kinematics. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  C L MacKenzie,et al.  Is object texture a constraint on human prehension?: kinematic evidence. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Alan Wing,et al.  Coordination of hand aperture with the spatial path of hand transport , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Miya K. Rand,et al.  Effect of speed manipulation on the control of aperture closure during reach-to-grasp movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Jiping He,et al.  Control of hand orientation and arm movement during reach and grasp , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Temporal constraints in reaching and grasping behavior , 1990 .

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Influence of different types of grasping on the transport component of prehension movements , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Vision and Action: The Control of Grasping , 1990 .

[19]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. , 2001, Psychological review.

[20]  Christine L. MacKenzie,et al.  Functional relationships between grasp and transport components in a prehension task , 1990 .

[21]  A. Wing,et al.  Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  R. Bootsma,et al.  Accuracy demands in natural prehension , 1993 .

[23]  Yiannis Aloimonos,et al.  Vision and action , 1995, Image Vis. Comput..

[24]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Grip reorganization during wrist transport: the influence of an Altered aperture , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Perceptual Structures and Distributed Motor Control , 1981 .

[26]  R. Johansson,et al.  Visual size cues in the programming of manipulative forces during precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  R. Johansson,et al.  Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  E. Brenner,et al.  A new view on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.

[30]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Influence of object position and size on human prehension movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  P. Mamassian Prehension of objects oriented in three-dimensional space , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Coordination of arm and wrist motion during a reaching task , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  M. A. Arbib,et al.  Models of Trajectory Formation and Temporal Interaction of Reach and Grasp. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Integrated control of hand transport and orientation during prehension movements , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Goodale,et al.  A model of the coupling between grip aperture and hand transport during human prehension , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Ivan Toni,et al.  On orienting the hand to reach and grasp an object , 1996, Neuroreport.

[37]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  James R. Bloedel,et al.  Temporal control of the reach and grip components during a prehension task in humans , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  T. Milner,et al.  A model for the generation of movements requiring endpoint precision , 1992, Neuroscience.

[40]  P Haggard,et al.  On the hand transport component of prehensile movements. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[43]  S. Geisser,et al.  On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .

[44]  George E Stelmach,et al.  Effect of Orienting the Finger Opposition Space in the Control of Reach-to-Grasp Movements , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[45]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Orienting the finger opposition space during prehension movements. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[46]  J. R. Bloedel,et al.  Grasping component alterations and limb transport , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Does a complex model help to understand grasping? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Parallel, interdependent channels for location and orientation in sensorimotor transformations for reaching and grasping. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.