Biological motion perception links diverse facets of theory of mind during middle childhood.

Two cornerstones of social development--social perception and theory of mind--undergo brain and behavioral changes during middle childhood, but the link between these developing domains is unclear. One theoretical perspective argues that these skills represent domain-specific areas of social development, whereas other perspectives suggest that both skills may reflect a more integrated social system. Given recent evidence from adults that these superficially different domains may be related, the current study examined the developmental relation between these social processes in 52 children aged 7 to 12 years. Controlling for age and IQ, social perception (perception of biological motion in noise) was significantly correlated with two measures of theory of mind: one in which children made mental state inferences based on photographs of the eye region of the face and another in which children made mental state inferences based on stories. Social perception, however, was not correlated with children's ability to make physical inferences from stories about people. Furthermore, the mental state inference tasks were not correlated with each other, suggesting a role for social perception in linking various facets of theory of mind.

[1]  M. Pavlova Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Rory T. Devine,et al.  Silent films and strange stories: theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. , 2013, Child development.

[3]  Luke E. Miller,et al.  Individual differences in the perception of biological motion: Links to social cognition and motor imagery , 2013, Cognition.

[4]  L. Phillips,et al.  Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding , 2011, Cognition.

[5]  Donald W. Pfaff,et al.  Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  U. Frith,et al.  Revisiting the strange stories: revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. , 2009, Child development.

[7]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Developmental continuity in theory of mind: speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children and adults. , 2011, Child development.

[9]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. , 2010, Developmental science.

[11]  Scott A. Miller,et al.  Theory of Mind: Beyond the Preschool Years , 2012 .

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[13]  Alan S. Kaufman,et al.  Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition , 2014 .

[14]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Visual Recognition of Biological Motion is Impaired in Children With Autism , 2003, Psychological science.

[15]  J. Fleiss Statistical methods for rates and proportions , 1974 .

[16]  Aina Puce,et al.  Configural Processing of Biological Motion in Human Superior Temporal Sulcus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[18]  R. Saxe,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience of Theory of Mind , 2013 .

[19]  H. Toutenburg Fleiss, J. L.: Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. John Wiley & Sons, New York‐London‐Sydney‐Toronto 1973. XIII, 233 S. , 1974 .

[20]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. , 2001 .

[21]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Functional Heterogeneity and Convergence in the Right Temporoparietal Junction. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  H. Tager-Flusberg Evaluating the Theory-of-Mind Hypothesis of Autism , 2007 .

[23]  E. Bora,et al.  Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis , 2009, Schizophrenia Research.

[24]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Perception of Biological Motion in Schizophrenia and Healthy Individuals: A Behavioral and fMRI Study , 2011, PloS one.

[25]  K. Pelphrey,et al.  School-aged children exhibit domain-specific responses to biological motion , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[26]  J. Lange,et al.  A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: a longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Klaus Krippendorff,et al.  Answering the Call for a Standard Reliability Measure for Coding Data , 2007 .

[30]  James C. Thompson,et al.  Social-Network Complexity in Humans Is Associated With the Neural Response to Social Information , 2014, Psychological science.

[31]  K. Pelphrey,et al.  An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for thinking about thoughts. , 2012, Child development.

[33]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Long trajectory for the development of sensitivity to global and biological motion. , 2011, Developmental science.

[34]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Accentuation of Individual Differences in Social Competence During the Transition to Adolescence. , 2011, Journal of research on adolescence : the official journal of the Society for Research on Adolescence.

[35]  A. Klin,et al.  Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion , 2009, Nature.

[36]  Karl Verfaillie,et al.  Perception of biological motion: A stimulus set of human point-light actions , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[37]  D. Watling,et al.  Peer relations and the understanding of faux pas: longitudinal evidence for bidirectional associations. , 2011, Child development.