On the need for attention-aware systems: Measuring effects of interruption on task performance, error rate, and affective state
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Cacioppo,et al. Handbook Of Psychophysiology , 2019 .
[2] Brian P. Bailey,et al. A framework for specifying and monitoring user tasks , 2006, Comput. Hum. Behav..
[3] Brian P. Bailey,et al. Towards an index of opportunity: understanding changes in mental workload during task execution , 2004, CHI.
[4] Brian P. Bailey,et al. Categories & Subject Descriptors: H.5.2 [Information , 2022 .
[5] Jacob P. Somervell,et al. A model for notification systems evaluation—assessing user goals for multitasking activity , 2003, TCHI.
[6] Eric Horvitz,et al. Learning and reasoning about interruption , 2003, ICMI '03.
[7] J. Gregory Trafton,et al. Preparing to resume an interrupted task: effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[8] John T. Stasko,et al. Establishing tradeoffs that leverage attention for utility: empirically evaluating information display in notification systems , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[9] Jeffrey S. Shell,et al. Interacting with groups of computers , 2003, Commun. ACM.
[10] Christopher A. Monk,et al. The Attentional Costs of Interrupting Task Performance at Various Stages , 2002 .
[11] Mary Czerwinski,et al. Scope: providing awareness of multiple notifications at a glance , 2002, AVI '02.
[12] Kara A. Latorella,et al. The Scope and Importance of Human Interruption in Human-Computer Interaction Design , 2002, Hum. Comput. Interact..
[13] J. Gregory Trafton,et al. Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[14] Blair MacIntyre,et al. Support for multitasking and background awareness using interactive peripheral displays , 2001, UIST '01.
[15] D. Meyer,et al. Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Thomas Jackson,et al. The cost of email interruption , 2001, Journal of Systems and Information Technology.
[17] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] Mary Czerwinski,et al. Notification, Disruption, and Memory: Effects of Messaging Interruptions on Memory and Performance , 2001, INTERACT.
[19] John T. Stasko,et al. Evaluating Animation in the Periphery as a Mechanism for Maintaining Awarness , 2001, INTERACT.
[20] Brian P. Bailey,et al. The Effects of Interruptions on Task Performance, Annoyance, and Anxiety in the User Interface , 2001, INTERACT.
[21] Mary Czerwinski,et al. Instant Messaging and Interruption: Influence of Task Type on Performance , 2000 .
[22] Paul P. Maglio,et al. Tradeoffs in displaying peripheral information , 2000, CHI.
[23] Mary Czerwinski,et al. Effects of instant messaging interruptions on computing tasks , 2000, CHI Extended Abstracts.
[24] Mary Czerwinski,et al. Instant Messaging: Effects of Relevance and Timing , 2000 .
[25] Eric Horvitz,et al. Attention-Sensitive Alerting , 1999, UAI.
[26] Joseph S. Valacich,et al. The Influence of Task Interruption on Individual Decision Making: An Information Overload Perspective , 1999 .
[27] Paul E. Spector,et al. Stress in the workplace: a comparison of gender and occupations , 1999 .
[28] Daniel C. McFarlane,et al. Coordinating the Interruption of People in Human-Computer Interaction , 1999, INTERACT.
[29] A Latorella Kara,et al. Effects of Modality on Interrupted Flight Deck Performance: Implications for Data Link , 1998 .
[30] John L. Sibert,et al. Heart rate variability: indicator of user state as an aid to human-computer interaction , 1998, CHI.
[31] J. Ellis. Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. , 1996 .
[32] F. Zijlstra,et al. Temporal factors in mental work: Effects of interrupted activities , 1999 .
[33] Pattie Maes,et al. Agents that reduce work and information overload , 1994, CACM.
[34] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[35] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[36] A Mital,et al. Noise in multiple-workstation open-plan computer rooms: measurements and annoyance. , 1992, Journal of human ergology.
[37] D. Damos. Multiple-task performance , 2020 .
[38] D. Broadbent,et al. What makes interruptions disruptive? A study of length, similarity, and complexity , 1989 .
[39] William Buxton,et al. Chunking and Phrasing and the Design of Human-Computer Dialogues (Invited Paper) , 1995, IFIP Congress.
[40] Donald A. Norman,et al. User Centered System Design: New Perspectives on Human-Computer Interaction , 1988 .
[41] S. J. Motowidlo,et al. Occupational stress: its causes and consequences for job performance. , 1986, The Journal of applied psychology.
[42] Donald A. Norman,et al. The control of multiple activities , 1986 .
[43] Dimitre Novatchev,et al. Chunking and Phrasing and the Design of Human-Computer Dialogues - Response , 1986, IFIP Congress.
[44] J. G. Hollands,et al. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .
[45] Allen Newell,et al. The psychology of human-computer interaction , 1983 .
[46] J. Beatty. Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.
[47] J. G. Kreifeldt,et al. Interruption as a test of the user-computer interface , 1981 .
[48] G. S. Sanders,et al. The motivating effects of distraction on task performance. , 1975 .
[49] C. Spielberger,et al. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .