Genetic and environmental mediation of the relationship between language and nonverbal impairment in 4-year-old twins.

This study of 4-year-old twins investigated the genetic and environmental origins of comorbidity between language impairment and nonverbal ability by testing the extent to which language impairment in one twin predicted nonverbal ability in the co-twin. Impairment of language ability was defined as scores below the 15th percentile on a general language scale derived from a battery of diverse language tests. Four hundred thirty-six children, members of 160 monozygotic (MZ) and 131 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs, were identified as language impaired. Language-impaired probands also suffered significant impairments in nonverbal ability. DeFries-Fulker extremes analysis showed evidence for substantial genetic mediation of the phenotypic relationship between language impairment and poor nonverbal ability in that language problems in one twin predicted poor nonverbal ability in the co-twin, much more so for MZ twins than for DZ twins. This finding held even when we excluded those children with language impairment whose nonverbal score indicated general cognitive delay. These results point to a general genetic factor that includes both language and nonverbal problems.

[1]  E. Viding,et al.  Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[2]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Twins and non-twin siblings: different estimates of shared environmental influence in early childhood. , 2003, Twin research : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[3]  R. Plomin,et al.  Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A Multivariate, Longitudinal Genetic Investigation of Language, Cognition and Behavior Problems in Childhood , 2002, Twin Research.

[4]  R. Plomin,et al.  Genetics and the development of language disabilities and abilities , 2002 .

[5]  Robert Plomin,et al.  The structure of language abilities at 4 years: a twin study. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[6]  R. Plomin,et al.  Associations between behaviour problems and verbal and nonverbal cognitive abilities and disabilities in early childhood. , 2002, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  I. Craig,et al.  Language-impaired children: No sign of the FOXP2 mutation , 2002, Neuroreport.

[8]  G Baird,et al.  FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language impairment. , 2002, American journal of human genetics.

[9]  F. Vargha-Khadem,et al.  Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  K. Stromswold The Heritability of Language: A Review and Metaanalysis of Twin, Adoption, and Linkage Studies , 2001 .

[11]  A. Monaco,et al.  A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder , 2001, Nature.

[12]  S. Purcell,et al.  Comorbidity between verbal and non‐verbal cognitive delays in 2‐year‐olds: a bivariate twin analysis , 2001 .

[13]  L B Leonard,et al.  Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[14]  R. Plomin,et al.  Lexical and grammatical development: a behavioural genetic perspective , 2000, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  R. Plomin,et al.  Genetic and environmental covariation between verbal and nonverbal cognitive development in infancy. , 2000, Child development.

[16]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Lessons from children with specific language impairment , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  H. V. D. Lely,et al.  Evidence for a grammar-specific deficit in children , 1998, Current Biology.

[18]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Bishop,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology and Developmental Disorders: Uncomfortable Bedfellows , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  Laurence B. Leonard,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 1997 .

[21]  Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al.  Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children , 1997 .

[22]  S. Petrill Molarity Versus Modularity of Cognitive Functioning? , 1997 .

[23]  C Donlan,et al.  Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[24]  D V Bishop,et al.  Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[25]  C Donlan,et al.  GENETIC BASIS OF SPECIFIC LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT: EVIDENCE FROM A TWIN STUDY , 1995, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[26]  D. Bishop,et al.  Is specific language impairment a valid diagnostic category? Genetic and psycholinguistic evidence. , 1994, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. , 1994, Memory.

[28]  G. Mcclearn,et al.  Is there G beyond g? (Is there genetic influence on specific cognitive abilities independent of genetic influence on general cognitive ability?) , 1994 .

[29]  D. Francis,et al.  The Validity of Discrepancy-Based Definitions of Reading Disabilities , 1992, Journal of learning disabilities.

[30]  Richard K. Olson,et al.  The External Validity of Age- Versus IQ-Discrepancy Definitions of Reading Disability , 1992, Journal of learning disabilities.

[31]  B A Lewis,et al.  A study of developmental speech and language disorders in twins. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[32]  P. Dale,et al.  Stability of the intelligence quotient-language quotient relation: is discrepancy modeling based on a myth? , 1992, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[33]  M. Rutter,et al.  Annotation: growing up as a twin: twin-singleton differences in psychological development. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  P. Tallal,et al.  Familial aggregation in specific language impairment. , 1989, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[35]  J B Tomblin,et al.  Familial concentration of developmental language impairment. , 1989, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[36]  D. Fulker,et al.  Multiple regression analysis of twin data: etiology of deviant scores versus individual differences. , 1988, Acta geneticae medicae et gemellologiae.

[37]  L. Thompson,et al.  The Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development: An Adoption Study of Two and Three-year olds , 1988 .

[38]  Dorothy M. Aram,et al.  Family History of Children with Developmental Language Disorders , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[39]  D. Fulker,et al.  Multiple regression analysis of twin data , 1985, Behavior genetics.

[40]  Matt McGue,et al.  Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex , 1984, Behavior genetics.

[41]  P Tallal,et al.  Selection of children with specific language deficits. , 1981, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[42]  N. Martin,et al.  An application of the analysis of covariance structures to the psychogenetical study of impulsiveness , 1977 .

[43]  J. Stevenson,et al.  The Prevalence of Language Delay in a Population of Three‐year‐old Children and its Association with General Retardation , 1976, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[44]  D. Fulker,et al.  Multivariate genetic analysis of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised (WISC-R) factors , 1995, Behavior genetics.

[45]  R. Olson,et al.  Colorado Reading Project: An update. , 1991 .

[46]  D. Sherman,et al.  Review of Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. , 1970 .