Movement Strategy Discovery during Training via Haptic Guidance

Haptic guidance has previously been investigated to facilitate motor skill training, whereupon a robotic device assists a trainee in executing the desired movement. However, many studies have reported a null or even detrimental effect of haptic guidance on training compared to unassisted practice. While prior studies have focused on using haptic guidance to refine a movement strategy, our study explores its effect on the discovery of a new strategy. Subjects learned to manipulate a virtual under-actuated system via a haptic device either with or without haptic guidance (and without haptic feedback of system dynamics). The guidance enabled subjects to experience a range of successful movements, rather than strictly enforcing one trajectory. Subjects who trained with guidance adopted a strategy that involved faster reaches, required greater control of the system's degrees of freedom, and increased the potential for faster task completion. However, overall improvement of task performance was limited with the new strategy.

[1]  Kenji Suzuki,et al.  Effect of Haptic Assistance on Learning Vehicle Reverse Parking Skills , 2014, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[2]  Ilana Nisky,et al.  Robot-assisted surgery: an emerging platform for human neuroscience research , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[3]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Experimentally confirmed mathematical model for human control of a non-rigid object. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  J Liu,et al.  Learning to perform a new movement with robotic assistance: comparison of haptic guidance and visual demonstration , 2006, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[5]  M. Sile O'Modhrain,et al.  The Virtual Teacher , 1999 .

[6]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Biological Learning and Control: How the Brain Builds Representations, Predicts Events, and Makes Decisions , 2012 .

[7]  Cagatay Basdogan,et al.  Intention Recognition for Dynamic Role Exchange in Haptic Collaboration , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[8]  G Wulf,et al.  Physical-guidance benefits in learning a complex motor skill. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  E Bizzi,et al.  Augmented Feedback Presented in a Virtual Environment Accelerates Learning of a Difficult Motor Task. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  J. Patton,et al.  Custom-designed haptic training for restoring reaching ability to individuals with poststroke hemiparesis. , 2006, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[11]  J. Patton,et al.  Can Robots Help the Learning of Skilled Actions? , 2009, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[12]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Optimal Control Predicts Human Performance on Objects with Internal Degrees of Freedom , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[13]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Rehabilitation Robotics: Performance-Based Progressive Robot-Assisted Therapy , 2003, Auton. Robots.

[14]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interference in Learning Internal Models of Inverse Dynamics in Humans , 1994, NIPS.

[15]  K. Newell Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[17]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor System , 2005, Neural Computation.

[18]  Abhishek Gupta,et al.  Shared Control in Haptic Systems for Performance Enhancement and Training , 2006 .

[19]  R. Riener,et al.  Patient-cooperative strategies for robot-aided treadmill training: first experimental results , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[20]  J. Patton,et al.  Interactive priming enhanced by negative damping aids learning of an object manipulation task , 2007, 2007 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[21]  C. Winstein,et al.  Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1994, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[22]  R. Riener,et al.  Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: A review , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[23]  S. Coquillart,et al.  Haptic Guidance Improves the Visuo-Manual Tracking of Trajectories , 2008, PloS one.

[24]  Marcia K. O'Malley,et al.  On the Efficacy of Haptic Guidance Schemes for Human Motor Learning , 2009 .

[25]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Acquisition of novel and complex motor skills: stable solutions where intrinsic noise matters less. , 2014, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[26]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Marcia Kilchenman O'Malley,et al.  Identifying Successful Motor Task Completion via Motion-Based Performance Metrics , 2014, IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems.

[28]  D. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Review of control strategies for robotic movement training after neurologic injury , 2009, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[29]  C. Shea,et al.  Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Daniel Vélez Día,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 2013 .

[31]  Frank Tendick,et al.  Haptic guidance: experimental evaluation of a haptic training method for a perceptual motor skill , 2002, Proceedings 10th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. HAPTICS 2002.

[32]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Perspectives and problems in motor learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  P. Wiseman The Virtual Teacher , 1998 .

[34]  Heather Carnahan,et al.  Motor Learning Perspectives on Haptic Training for the Upper Extremities , 2014, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[35]  Christopher D Mah,et al.  Manipulating objects with internal degrees of freedom: evidence for model-based control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Haptic Guidance Can Enhance Motor Learning of a Steering Task , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  Marcia Kilchenman O'Malley,et al.  The Task-Dependent Efficacy of Shared-Control Haptic Guidance Paradigms , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[38]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Frequent Augmented Feedback Can Degrade Learning: Evidence and Interpretations , 1991 .

[39]  J. Krakauer,et al.  How is a motor skill learned? Change and invariance at the levels of task success and trajectory control. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Robotic guidance benefits the learning of dynamic, but not of spatial movement characteristics , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Comparison of error-amplification and haptic-guidance training techniques for learning of a timing-based motor task by healthy individuals , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Seungmoon Choi,et al.  Effects of haptic guidance and disturbance on motor learning: Potential advantage of haptic disturbance , 2010, 2010 IEEE Haptics Symposium.

[43]  Mark Mulder,et al.  Neuromuscular Analysis as a Guideline in designing Shared Control , 2010 .

[44]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  The role of haptic feedback when manipulating nonrigid objects. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  D.J. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Robot-enhanced motor learning: accelerating internal model formation during locomotion by transient dynamic amplification , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[46]  Marcia K. O'Malley,et al.  Shared Control for Training in Virtual Environments: Learning Through Demonstration? , 2006 .