Are Names Difficult to Recall because They Are Unique? A Case Study of a Patient with Anomia

According to Burton and Bruce (1992), names are more difficult to recall than biographical information about people, such as their occupation, because names are unique or highly distinctive. It follows from this that anomic patients who have great difficulty in recalling names should also find it difficult to recall other information that is unique to a particular individual. This paper attempts to evaluate this claim by examining the case of NP, a patient who has severe anomic word-finding difficulties following the rupture and repair of a posterior cerebral artery aneurysm. NP's ability to recall biographical information about people that she cannot name was investigated in a series of experiments. These revealed that she can answer specific questions about the occupations and appearance of well-known people and can recall distinctive meaningful information about them such as the identity of their spouse, even though she is unable to recall their name. It is argued that these results support the view that names are represented in a store separate from that for semantic information about people that we know. The findings are therefore consistent with the sequential stage model of face identification put forward by Bruce and Young (1986) and are explained in terms of the theory of speech production put forward by Levelt (1989).

[1]  Janice Kay,et al.  Face processing and name retrieval in an anomic aphasic: Names are stored separately from semantic information about familiar people , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[3]  A. M. Burton,et al.  I recognize your face but I can't remember your name: a simple explanation? , 1992, British journal of psychology.

[4]  A. Young,et al.  Impaired memory for new visual forms. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  C. Semenza,et al.  Evidence from aphasia for the role of proper names as pure referring expressions , 1989, Nature.

[6]  Tim Valentine,et al.  What's in a name? access to information from people's names , 1991 .

[7]  R A Johnston,et al.  Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. , 1990, British journal of psychology.

[8]  J. Hanley,et al.  Access to visual information from a name is contingent on access to identity-specific semantic information. , 1993, Memory.

[9]  J. Hanley,et al.  The effects of different types of retrieval cues on the recall of names of famous faces , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[10]  A. Ellis,et al.  A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Implications for psychological models of word retrieval. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  A. Young,et al.  Dissociable face processing impairments after brain injury. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[12]  A. Young,et al.  Accessing stored information about familiar people , 1988 .

[13]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  A. Young,et al.  The faces that launched a thousand slips: everyday difficulties and errors in recognizing people. , 1985, British journal of psychology.

[15]  Carlo Semenza Generating proper names: a case of selective inability , 1996 .

[16]  J. Richard Hanley,et al.  Defective recognition of familiar people , 1989 .

[17]  A. Young,et al.  Understanding face recognition. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[18]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? , 1991 .

[19]  A W Ellis,et al.  Routes through the Face Recognition System , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  Tim Valentine,et al.  From Monroe to Moreau: An analysis of face naming errors , 1992, Cognition.

[21]  David Howard,et al.  On the Origin of Semantic Errors in Naming: Evidence from the Case of a Global Aphasic , 1984 .

[22]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[23]  J. Hanley,et al.  Perception and memory for faces in Parkinson's disease , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  E K Warrington,et al.  Testing for nominal dysphasia. , 1980, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[25]  Edward H.F. de Haan,et al.  A dissociation between the sense of familiarity and access to semantic information concerning familiar people , 1991 .

[26]  C Semenza,et al.  Production of proper names: a clinical case study of the effects of phonemic cueing. , 1993, Memory.

[27]  E M Critchley,et al.  Loss of memory for people following temporal lobe damage. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[29]  A. Young,et al.  Putting names to faces , 1987 .

[30]  R. Carney,et al.  Prosopanomia? a possible category-specific anomia for faces , 1993 .

[31]  Gillian Cohen,et al.  Why is it difficult to put names to faces , 1990 .

[32]  V. Bruce,et al.  Lost properties? Retrieval differences between name codes and semantic codes for familiar people , 1990 .

[33]  Karalyn Patterson,et al.  Letter-by-letter Reading: Psychological Descriptions of a Neurologial Syndrome , 1982 .

[34]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[35]  E. Renzi,et al.  Proper Name Anomia , 1992, Cortex.

[36]  V. Bruce,et al.  Resolving semantically induced tip-of-the-tongue states for proper nouns , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[37]  L Cipolotti,et al.  Spared written naming of proper nouns: a case report. , 1993, Memory.

[38]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Generating proper names: A case of selective inability , 1988 .