Psychometric properties of conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation indices: response time and conflict N2 event-related potentials.

We assessed the psychometric properties, including internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and number of trials needed for stability, for response time (RT) and conflict N2 amplitude measures of conflict adaptation (i.e., sequential trial or Gratton effects). Event-related potentials (ERPs) were obtained from 79 healthy individuals (44 female) during completion of a modified Eriksen flanker task. RTs and N2 amplitude showed reliable conflict adaptation (i.e., previous-trial congruency influenced current-trial measures). Conflict adaptation indices were stable as indicated by reliable internal consistency and 2-week test-retest stability. The number of trials needed for conservative, reliable RT and N2 amplitude measurements was 29 and 30, respectively. This investigation empirically establishes that these conflict adaptation indices represent robust, dependable measures.

[1]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[2]  Joseph Dien,et al.  Separating the visual sentence N400 effect from the P400 sequential expectancy effect: Cognitive and neuroanatomical implications , 2010, Brain Research.

[3]  Tobias Egner,et al.  The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[4]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Cognitive performance and electrophysiological indices of cognitive control: a validation study of conflict adaptation. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Retest reliability of medial frontal negativities during performance monitoring. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Gene A. Brewer,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Variation in Working Memory Capacity and Cognitive Control: Goal Maintenance and Microadjustments of Control , 2022 .

[8]  Anna Christina Nobre,et al.  Anticipating Conflict Facilitates Controlled Stimulus-response Selection , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  A. Freitas,et al.  When cognitive control is calibrated: event-related potential correlates of adapting to information-processing conflict despite erroneous response preparation. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[10]  Michael J. Larson,et al.  Cognitive Control and Conflict Adaptation Similarities in Children and Adults , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.

[11]  S. Johnstone,et al.  Varying required effort during interference control in children with AD/HD: task performance and ERPs. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring , 2006, Psychological science.

[13]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Mediates Individual Differences in Conflict-driven Cognitive Control , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Michael J. Larson,et al.  Sex differences in electrophysiological indices of conflict monitoring , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[18]  E. Awh,et al.  The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation , 2009, Psychological research.

[19]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in reward and conflict processing as revealed by the feedback error-related negativity and N200 , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[20]  Greg Hajcak,et al.  The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[21]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Performance monitoring and the medial prefrontal cortex: a review of individual differences and context effects as a window on self-regulation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[23]  M. Junghöfer,et al.  The polar average reference effect: a bias in estimating the head surface integral in EEG recording , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  T. Banaschewski,et al.  Action Monitoring in Boys With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Their Nonaffected Siblings, and Normal Control Subjects: Evidence for an Endophenotype , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  On the number of trials necessary for stabilization of error-related brain activity across the life span. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[27]  D. Cicchetti Methodological Commentary The Precision of Reliability and Validity Estimates Re-Visited: Distinguishing Between Clinical and Statistical Significance of Sample Size Requirements , 2001 .

[28]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  The effects of induced state negative affect on performance monitoring processes. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  How does noise affect amplitude and latency measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs)? A methodological critique and simulation study. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[30]  Darren George,et al.  SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference , 1998 .

[31]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Effects of repetition priming on electrophysiological and behavioral indices of conflict adaptation and cognitive control. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[32]  Aziz M. Ulug,et al.  Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[33]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  The reliability of ERP components in the auditory oddball paradigm. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Neuroelectric and behavioral indices of interference control during acute cycling , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[35]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization. , 2007, Developmental science.

[36]  E. Soetens,et al.  Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[38]  John G. Kerns,et al.  Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[39]  P. Liddle,et al.  Event-Related Potentials in Adolescents with Schizophrenia and Their Siblings: A Comparison with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[40]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  P. Zelazo,et al.  What is Cognitive Control , 2013 .

[43]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  A lifespan comparison of the reliability, test-retest stability, and signal-to-noise ratio of event-related potentials assessed during performance monitoring. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[45]  Scott A. Baldwin,et al.  Performance monitoring following conflict: Internal adjustments in cognitive control? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[47]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Cognitive control in mild traumatic brain injury: conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  L. Jonkman,et al.  Methylphenidate improves deficient error evaluation in children with ADHD: An event-related brain potential study , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[49]  G. Hajcak,et al.  Reliability of error-related brain activity , 2009, Brain Research.

[50]  Jan R. Wessel,et al.  Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[51]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility in Huntington's disease: a cognitive-neurophysiological analysis. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[53]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  The Impact of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase and Dopamine D4 Receptor Genotypes on Neurophysiological Markers of Performance Monitoring , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  R. West,et al.  Sensitivity of medial frontal cortex to response and nonresponse conflict. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Nigel O'Brian,et al.  Generalizability Theory I , 2003 .

[56]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[57]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Biological Psychology , 2022 .

[58]  A. Anastasi Psychological testing, 6th ed. , 1988 .

[59]  Scott A Baldwin,et al.  Temporal stability of the error-related negativity (ERN) and post-error positivity (Pe): the role of number of trials. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[60]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Parametric Manipulation of the Conflict Signal and Control-state Adaptation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  J. Os,et al.  Auditory P300 and N100 components as intermediate phenotypes for psychotic disorder: Familial liability and reliability , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[63]  T. Banaschewski,et al.  Performance monitoring is altered in adult ADHD: A familial event-related potential investigation , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  S. Strakowski,et al.  Conflict monitoring and adaptation in individuals at familial risk for developing bipolar disorder , 2013, Bipolar disorders.

[65]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Cognitive control and conflict adaptation in youth with high-functioning autism. , 2012, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.