A Model of Positive Self-Image in Subjective Assessments

This paper suggests a mechanism that describes individuals' positive self-image in subjective assessments of their relative abilities. The mechanism assumes individuals have heterogeneous production functions that determine ability as a function of multiple skills; make skill-enhancing investments with the goal of maximizing their ability; and make ability comparisons using their own production function. Within this framework, the paper provides conditions under which there is positive self-image. Positive self-image is increasing in the ease of the task, the number of different skills needed for the task, and the variability of production technologies in the population.

[1]  D. Dunning,et al.  Self-serving prototypes of social categories. , 1991, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

[2]  O. Svenson ARE WE ALL LESS RISKY AND MORE SKILLFUL THAN OUR FELLOW DRIVERS , 1981 .

[3]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Calibration of probabilities: the state of the art to 1980 , 1982 .

[4]  Andrew Postlewaite,et al.  Confidence-Enhanced Performance , 2001 .

[5]  J. Kruger,et al.  Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  L. Abramson,et al.  Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser? , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  J. Tirole,et al.  Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation , 2002 .

[8]  J. Kruger Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  D. Dunning,et al.  Construal processes and trait ambiguity: implications for self-peer agreement in personality judgment. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  D. Dunning,et al.  Ambiguity and self-evaluation: the role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability , 1989 .

[11]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: When Predictions Fail: The Dilemma of Unrealistic Optimism , 2002 .

[12]  Tai Gyu Kim,et al.  Myopic social prediction and the solo comparison effect. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Overconfident: Do You Put Your Money on it? , 2005 .

[14]  E. Steen Rational Overoptimism (and Other Biases) , 2004 .

[15]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[16]  D. Sarel,et al.  Nonunique invulnerability : Singular versus distributional probabilities and unrealistic optimism in comparative risk judgments , 1996 .

[17]  George A. Akerlof,et al.  The Economic Consequences of Cognitive Dissonance , 1982 .

[18]  Joachim Krueger,et al.  Unskilled, unaware, or both? The better-than-average heuristic and statistical regression predict errors in estimates of own performance. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Thomas B. Astebro,et al.  Inventor Perseverance after Being Told to Quit : The Role of Cognitive Biases , 2007 .

[20]  David Dunning,et al.  Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment , 1996 .

[21]  L. Bauman,et al.  Misperception Among Gay Men of the Risk for AIDS Associated with Their Sexual Behavior1 , 1987 .

[22]  Zafer Akin Time Inconsistency and Learning in Bargaining Games , 2005 .

[23]  B. Kőszegi EGO UTILITY, OVERCONFIDENCE, AND TASK CHOICE , 2006 .

[24]  K. Cross Not can, but will college teaching be improved? , 1977 .

[25]  G. Regehr,et al.  Difficulties in Recognizing One's Own Incompetence: Novice Physicians Who Are Unskilled and Unaware of It , 2001, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[26]  A Model of Rational Bias in Self-Assessments , 2003 .

[27]  M. Alicke Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. , 1985 .

[28]  R. Felson Ambiguity and bias in the self-concept. , 1981 .

[29]  D. Dunning,et al.  The More Rational Side of Self-Serving Prototypes: The Effects of Success and Failure Performance Feedback , 1998 .

[30]  J. Ouellette,et al.  Abandoning Unrealistic Optimism: Performance Estimates and the Temporal Proximity of Self-Relevant Feedback , 1996 .

[31]  Hans K. Hvide Pragmatic Beliefs and Overconfidence , 2002 .

[32]  J. Tirole,et al.  Self Confidence: Intrapersonal Strategies , 2000 .

[33]  Chris Guthrie,et al.  Inside the Judicial Mind , 2001 .

[34]  W. Klein,et al.  Exaggerated Self-Assessments and the Preference for Controllable Risks , 1994 .

[35]  B Fischhoff,et al.  Perceived driving safety and seatbelt usage. , 1985, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[36]  Aviad Heifetz,et al.  On the Evolutionary Emergence of Optimism , 2000 .

[37]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social psychology , 2019, Islamic Psychology.

[38]  E. Steen Skill or Luck? Biases of Rational Agents , 2002 .

[39]  A. Edlin,et al.  Strict Monotonicity in Comparative Statics , 1998 .

[40]  D. Dunning Words to live by: The self and definitions of social concepts and categories. , 1993 .

[41]  I. Brocas,et al.  Biases in Perceptions, Beliefs and Behavior , 2004 .

[42]  M. Rabin Cognitive Dissonance and Social Change , 1994 .

[43]  W. Klein MAINTAINING SELF-SERVING SOCIAL COMPARISONS: ATTENUATING THE PERCEIVED SIGNIFICANCE OF RISK-INCREASING BEHAVIORS , 1996 .

[44]  David M. Messick,et al.  On Being Better but not Smarter than Others: The Muhammad Ali Effect , 1989 .

[45]  Corporate Downsizing to Rebuild Team Spirit: How Costly Voting Can Foster Cooperation , 2007 .

[46]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[47]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Overconfidence and Excess Entry: An Experimental Approach , 1999 .