The onset of tense marking in children at risk for specific language impairment.

PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to develop and evaluate measures reflecting the onset of tense marking for children between the ages of 2;0 (years;months) and 3;0. METHOD The validity of 4 cumulative measures of tense marker emergence and productivity was evaluated relative to existing measures of early grammatical development in a sample of 20 children followed longitudinally. Fourteen children were at risk for specific language impairment (AR-SLI group), and 6 children had low average language abilities (LA group). RESULTS All measures of onset were highly correlated with the traditional measures; however, children's progress toward mastery of grammatical tense marking was best explained by the productivity of their tense marking systems. Finally, the onset measures imposing productivity requirements best differentiated children in the LA group from those in the AR-SLI group. CONCLUSIONS The clinical implications for using the late onset of tense marking to improve the early identification of SLI are discussed.

[1]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Child Language Data Exchange System: an update , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[2]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Late talkers at 2: outcome at age 3. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[3]  L B Leonard,et al.  Grammatical morphology and the lexicon in children with specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[4]  Y. Levy,et al.  Language Competence Across Populations: Towards a definition of Specific Language Impairment , 2003 .

[5]  R. Brown A First Language , 1973 .

[6]  R S Chapman,et al.  The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. , 1981, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[7]  L B Leonard,et al.  Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: a discriminant function analysis. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[8]  K. Wexler,et al.  Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. , 1996, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[9]  Pamela A. Hadley,et al.  Early verb-related vulnerability among children with specific language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[10]  P. Hadley,et al.  Individual differences in the onset of tense marking: a growth-curve analysis. , 2006, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[11]  C. Dollaghan,et al.  Grammatical morpheme production in 4-year-old children. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[12]  J B Tomblin,et al.  A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. , 1996, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[13]  Jon F. Miller,et al.  Assessing language production in children , 1980 .

[14]  Laurence B. Leonard,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 1997 .

[15]  L. Rescorla,et al.  The Language Development Survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. , 1989, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[16]  P. Dale,et al.  Productivity of Emerging Word Combinations in Toddlers With Specific Expressive Language Impairment , 1997 .

[17]  K. Wexler,et al.  Family histories of children with SLI who show extended optional infinitives. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[18]  H. Scarborough Index of Productive Syntax , 1990, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[19]  Pamela Ann Hadley,et al.  Emergent Uses of BE and DO: Evidence from Children with Specific Language Impairment. , 1996 .

[20]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Minimalist Program , 1992 .

[21]  Rebecca J. McCauley,et al.  Assessment of Language Disorders in Children , 2000 .

[22]  Susan A. Sullivan,et al.  Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised , 1998 .

[23]  M L Rice,et al.  Tense over time: the longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[24]  L. Leonard,et al.  Functional Categories and Specific Language Impairment: A Case Study. , 1995 .

[25]  A. Williams,et al.  A Prospective Longitudinal Study of Phonological Development in Late Talkers. , 2003, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[26]  T. Klee Developmental and diagnostic characteristics of quantitative measures of children's language production , 1992 .

[27]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Verb use in specific language impairment. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[28]  Steven H. Long,et al.  Accuracy of Four Language Analysis Procedures Performed Automatically , 2001 .

[29]  Matthew Rispoli When children reach beyond their grasp: why some children make pronoun case errors and others don't , 2005, Journal of Child Language.

[30]  Stephen Wilson Lexically specific constructions in the acquisition of inflection in English , 2003, Journal of Child Language.

[31]  B. Lewis,et al.  A Clinical Tool for Evaluating the Familial Basis of Speech and Language Disorders , 1993 .

[32]  M L Rice,et al.  Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[33]  L. Bloom,et al.  Language development and language disorders , 1979 .

[34]  Geralyn R. Timler,et al.  Recommending Intervention for Toddlers With Specific Language Learning Difficulties , 1998 .

[35]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[36]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[37]  L. Singh,et al.  Vocabulary growth in late talkers: lexical development from 2;0 to 3;0 , 2000, Journal of Child Language.