Aiming accuracy in preferred and non-preferred limbs: implications for programing models of motor control

Most models of motor programing contend that one can perform learned actions with different muscle groups or limbs demonstrating the concept of motor equivalence. The goal of this review is to determine the generality of this concept within the context of aiming movements performed by both preferred and non-preferred limbs. Theoretical approaches to motor programing are described, followed by a comparison of a variety of kinematic measures taken from preferred and non-preferred limbs from simple and more complex aiming tasks. In general, the support for motor equivalency is strong for one- and two-dimensional aiming tasks and for simultaneous bimanual movements, but mixed for unconstrained throwing tasks and tasks that require feedback-based corrections.

[1]  E. A. Roy,et al.  Kinematic Analyses of Manual Asymmetries in Visual Aiming Movements , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  C Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  A. Skurvydas,et al.  Behavior of dominant and non dominant arms during ballistic protractive target-directed movements , 2009, Human Physiology.

[4]  Carol A. Putnam,et al.  On the Space-Time Structure of Human Interlimb Co-Ordination , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[5]  George E. Stelmach,et al.  Movement structure in young and elderly adults during goal-directed movements of the left and right arm , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[6]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[8]  J. Cooke,et al.  Initial agonist burst duration depends on movement amplitude , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: Implications for interhemispheric communication in normal brain , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[10]  Deborah L. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric control of the initial and corrective components of aiming movements , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb differences in control of movement extent. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  10 A Preliminary Theory of Two-Hand Cd-Ordinated Control , 1980 .

[13]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  The symmetry of interlimb transfer depends on workspace locations , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  J I Todor,et al.  Accommodation to increased accuracy demands by the right and left hands. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  Jacob S Nteere,et al.  Information Capacity of the Human Motor system , 1982 .

[16]  R. Emmerik,et al.  The effects of practice on limb kinematics in a throwing task. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  Anthony N. Carlsen,et al.  Response preparation changes following practice of an asymmetrical bimanual movement , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  W. Ritchie Russell,et al.  Studies in Neurology , 1949 .

[22]  J. Hore,et al.  Timing finger opening in overarm throwing based on a spatial representation of hand path. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Cooke,et al.  Matching of movements made independently by the two arms in normal humans. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  R. E. Hicks,et al.  Asmmetry of Bilateral Transfer. , 1974 .

[25]  K M Heilman,et al.  Left-hemisphere motor dominance in righthanders. , 1980, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[26]  D Tweed,et al.  Overarm throws with the nondominant arm: kinematics of accuracy. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  A. V. Van Gemmert,et al.  The direction of bilateral transfer depends on the performance parameter. , 2013, Human movement science.

[28]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Bimanual Movement Control: Information processing and Interaction Effects , 1984 .

[29]  B. Fowler,et al.  The Coordination of Bimanual Aiming Movements: Evidence for Progressive Desynchronization , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[30]  Jérôme Barral,et al.  Aiming in adults: Sex and laterality effects , 2004, Laterality.

[31]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.

[32]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  Pamela J Bryden,et al.  Pushing the Limits of Task Difficulty for the Right and Left Hands in Manual Aiming , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[34]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Left-Hemisphere Motor Dominance in Righthandersi , 1980, Cortex.

[35]  J. Hore,et al.  Control of joint rotations in overarm throws of different speeds made by dominant and nondominant arms. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  David E. Sherwood,et al.  Interlimb amplitude differences, spatial assimilations, and the temporal structure of rapid bimanual movements , 1994 .

[37]  M. Peters,et al.  Why the Preferred Hand Taps More Quickly than the Non-preferred Hand: Three Experiments on Handedness* , 1980 .

[38]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  R. Carson,et al.  Manual asymmetries: feedback processing, output variability, and spatial complexity-resolving some inconsistencies. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[40]  F. Bartlett,et al.  Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology , 1932 .

[41]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  J. Annett,et al.  The Control of Movement in the Preferred and Non-Preferred Hands* , 1979, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  D. Elliott,et al.  Sensory-motor equivalence: manual aiming in C6 tetraplegics following musculotendinous transfer surgery at the elbow , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  R. Schmidt A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. , 1975 .