An Environment-Wide Study of Adult Cognitive Performance in the 23andMe Cohort

With the emergence of web-based data collection methods, large digital health cohorts offer the opportunity to conduct behavioral and epidemiologic research at an unprecedented scale. The size and breadth of such data sets enable discovery of novel associations across the phenotypic spectrum.We deployed the digital symbol substitution test (DSST) online to consented 23andMe research participants 50-85 years of age. We tested cross-sectional associations between DSST performance and 824 phenotypes using linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, age*sex interaction, device, time of cohort entry, and ancestry, separately among discovery (n=144,786) and replication (n=93,428) samples; additional analyses further adjusted for education. We post-stratified association estimates on age, sex, and education to adjust for discrepancies across subsamples. Leveraging the rich genetic and phenotypic data available at 23andMe, we also estimated genetic and environmental correlations between DSST and its top correlates using linkage disequilibrium score regression.97 phenotypes were significantly (false discovery rate < 0.05) and strongly (standardized effect size > |0.5|) associated with DSST performance in the discovery phase. Of those, 60 (38 with additional adjustment for education) demonstrated both statistical significance and consistent direction of association in the replication sample. The significantly associated phenotypes largely clustered into the following categories: psychiatric traits (e.g. anxiety, β per 1 SD = −0.74, P-value=3.9×10−169), education (e.g. highest math class completed, β per 1 SD = 2.11, P-value<1 × 10−300), leisure activities (e.g. solitary activities like puzzles, β per 1 SD = 1.85, P-value<1.0×10−300), social determinants (e.g. household income, β per 1 SD = 1.20, P-value= 8.9×10−245, and lifestyle (e.g. years smoked, β per 1 SD = −0.98, P-value= 2.2×10−78). We identified several reproducible genetic correlations between DSST and its top associated exposures (e.g. 0.48 for leisure activities like puzzles, 0.28 for years of education, and −0.24 for anxiety; all P ≤ 7.9×10−26). For almost all exposures, genetic correlations with DSST were considerably stronger than environmental correlations.We have conducted the largest study of cognitive performance to date, building evidence supporting its correlations with many social, lifestyle, and clinical exposures. We established that the observed associations are in part underpinned by shared genetic architecture. Our study illustrates the potential of large-scale digital cohorts to contribute to epidemiologic discovery.

[1]  D. Jacobs,et al.  EARLY ADULT TO MID-LIFE CIGARETTE SMOKING AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION: FINDINGS FROM THE CARDIA STUDY , 2019, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[2]  Danny J. Smith,et al.  The associations between self-reported depression, self-reported chronic inflammatory conditions and cognitive abilities in UK Biobank , 2019, European Psychiatry.

[3]  S. Denaxas,et al.  The relationship between sleep duration, cognition and dementia: a Mendelian randomization study , 2019, International journal of epidemiology.

[4]  John P A Ioannidis,et al.  Signals Among Signals: Prioritizing Nongenetic Associations in Massive Data Sets , 2019, American journal of epidemiology.

[5]  K. Ressler,et al.  Social cognition or social class and culture? On the interpretation of differences in social cognitive performance , 2019, Psychological Medicine.

[6]  L. Germine,et al.  Digital neuropsychology: Challenges and opportunities at the intersection of science and software , 2019, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[7]  Judith Jaeger,et al.  Digit Symbol Substitution Test , 2018, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[8]  Jonathan P. Beauchamp,et al.  Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals , 2018, Nature Genetics.

[9]  T. Luck,et al.  Normative data for the Digit Symbol Substitution Test in a population-based sample aged 65–79 years: Results from the German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Adults (DEGS1) , 2018, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[10]  Kathryn E Kemper,et al.  Comparison of Genotypic and Phenotypic Correlations: Cheverud’s Conjecture in Humans , 2018, Genetics.

[11]  H. Walter,et al.  How Accumulated Real Life Stress Experience and Cognitive Speed Interact on Decision-Making Processes , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[12]  S. Studenski,et al.  Digit Symbol Substitution test and future clinical and subclinical disorders of cognition, mobility and mood in older adults. , 2016, Age and ageing.

[13]  Camille L. Ryan,et al.  Educational Attainment in the United States: 2015. Population Characteristics. Current Population Reports. P20-578. , 2016 .

[14]  N. Eriksson,et al.  GWAS of 89,283 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with self-reporting of being a morning person , 2016, Nature Communications.

[15]  J. Zajecka,et al.  A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled, Active-Reference, Double-Blind, Flexible-Dose Study of the Efficacy of Vortioxetine on Cognitive Function in Major Depressive Disorder , 2015, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[16]  J. Williamson,et al.  Heritability and genetic association analysis of cognition in the Diabetes Heart Study , 2014, Neurobiology of Aging.

[17]  H. Amièva,et al.  Compensatory mechanisms in higher-educated subjects with Alzheimer's disease: a study of 20 years of cognitive decline. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  M. Daly,et al.  LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies , 2014, Nature Genetics.

[19]  E. Tucker-Drob,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on Cognition Across Development and Context , 2013, Current directions in psychological science.

[20]  Y. Stern Cognitive Reserve: Implications for Assessment and Intervention , 2013, Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica.

[21]  Katherine M Flegal,et al.  Prevalence of obesity and trends in the distribution of body mass index among US adults, 1999-2010. , 2012, JAMA.

[22]  E. Turkheimer,et al.  Emergence of a Gene × Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Infant Mental Ability Between 10 Months and 2 Years , 2011, Psychological science.

[23]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Cognitive Reserve: Implications for Assessment and Intervention , 2013, Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica.

[24]  D. Dinges,et al.  Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. , 2007, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[25]  H. Amièva,et al.  Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population-based studies. , 2006, American journal of epidemiology.

[26]  T. Manolio,et al.  Education and the cognitive decline associated with MRI-defined brain infarct , 2006, Neurology.

[27]  M. Kotler,et al.  Digit Symbol Substitution Test Performance: Sex Differences in a Hebrew-Readers' Health Population , 2006, Perceptual and motor skills.

[28]  A. Malarcher,et al.  State-specific prevalence of cigarette smoking and quitting among adults--United States, 2004. , 2005, MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report.

[29]  D. Knopman,et al.  Cognitive function predicts first-time stroke and heart disease , 2005, Neurology.

[30]  Thomas Lumley,et al.  Analysis of Complex Survey Samples , 2004 .

[31]  Christina Wasylyshyn,et al.  Adult age and digit symbol substitution performance: a meta-analysis. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[32]  Jean-François Dartigues,et al.  The Effect of Education on Cognitive Performances and Its Implication for the Constitution of the Cognitive Reserve , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[33]  P. Lovibond,et al.  The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[34]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[35]  Daniel R. McLeod,et al.  An automated version of the digit symbol substitution test (DSST) , 1982 .

[36]  L. Suter Educational Attainment in the United States: March 1977 and 1976. Current Population Reports, Population Characteristics, Series P-20, No. 314. , 1977 .

[37]  A. B. Hill The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation? , 1965, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[38]  Austin Bradford Hill The environment and disease: association or causation? , 1965, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[39]  A. B. Hill,et al.  "The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation?" (1965), by Austin Bradford Hill , 2017 .

[40]  J. Zajecka,et al.  A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled, Active-Reference, Double-Blind, Flexible-Dose Study of the Efficacy of Vortioxetine on Cognitive Function in Major Depressive Disorder , 2016, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[41]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[42]  D. Carmelli,et al.  Performance on the digit symbol substitution test and 5-year mortality in the Western Collaborative Group Study. , 1995, American journal of epidemiology.

[43]  Wilford W. Spradlin,et al.  The Emergence of I , 1984 .