Two Routes to Expertise in Mental Rotation

The ability to imagine objects undergoing rotation (mental rotation) improves markedly with practice, but an explanation of this plasticity remains controversial. Some researchers propose that practice speeds up the rate of a general-purpose rotation algorithm. Others maintain that performance improvements arise through the adoption of a new cognitive strategy-repeated exposure leads to rapid retrieval from memory of the required response to familiar mental rotation stimuli. In two experiments we provide support for an integrated explanation of practice effects in mental rotation by combining behavioral and EEG measures in a way that provides more rigorous inference than is available from either measure alone. Before practice, participants displayed two well-established signatures of mental rotation: Both response time and EEG negativity increased linearly with rotation angle. After extensive practice with a small set of stimuli, both signatures of mental rotation had all but disappeared. In contrast, after the same amount of practice with a much larger set both signatures remained, even though performance improved markedly. Taken together, these results constitute a reversed association, which cannot arise from variation in a single cause, and so they provide compelling evidence for the existence of two routes to expertise in mental rotation. We also found novel evidence that practice with the large but not the small stimulus set increased the magnitude of an early visual evoked potential, suggesting increased rotation speed is enabled by improved efficiency in extracting three-dimensional information from two-dimensional stimuli.

[1]  Timothy C. Rickard A CMPL alternative account of practice effects in numerosity judgment tasks. , 1999 .

[2]  D. Uttal,et al.  The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[4]  D. Bamber State-trace analysis: A method of testing simple theories of causation , 1979 .

[5]  M. Tarr,et al.  Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  G. Wang,et al.  Three-dimensional object recognition learning alters an early ERP component of N1 , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Heil,et al.  Toward a chronopsychophysiology of mental rotation. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[8]  T. Rickard Bending the power law : A CMPL theory of strategy shifts and the automatization of cognitive skills , 1997 .

[9]  Richard De Lisi,et al.  Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance , 1996 .

[10]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The power law repealed: The case for an exponential law of practice , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Training generalized spatial skills , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  Martin Heil,et al.  The functional significance of ERP effects during mental rotation. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[13]  Daniel Andrew Bajic,et al.  The temporal dynamics of strategy execution in cognitive skill learning , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Event-related potentials during mental rotation: Disentangling the contributions of character classification and image transformation. , 1996 .

[15]  I. M. Harris,et al.  Selective right parietal lobe activation during mental rotation: a parametric PET study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Right Hemisphere , 1997, Brain and Language.

[17]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[18]  Fedor Jagla,et al.  Linking performance with brain potentials: Mental rotation-related negativity revisited , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  T. Rickard,et al.  Toward a generalized theory of the shift to retrieval in cognitive skill learning , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[20]  K. Kirsner,et al.  Discovering functionally independent mental processes: the principle of reversed association. , 1988, Psychological review.

[21]  R. Kail,et al.  Impact of practice on speed of mental rotation. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  M. Farah,et al.  Mental rotation: An event-related potential study with a validated mental rotation task , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[25]  J. Zacks Neuroimaging Studies of Mental Rotation: A Meta-analysis and Review , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  William S. Cleveland,et al.  Visualizing Data , 1993 .

[27]  J. Droulez,et al.  The influence of long-term practice on mental rotation of 3-D objects. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Cerebral processes during visuo-motor imagery of hands. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[29]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Measuring 3-D understanding on the Web and in the laboratory , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[30]  C. Pernet,et al.  Low and high imagers activate networks differentially in mental rotation , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  F. Rösler,et al.  What is improved if a mental rotation task is repeated – the efficiency of memory access, or the speed of a transformation routine? , 1998, Psychological research.

[33]  Patrick Berg,et al.  Artifact Correction of the Ongoing EEG Using Spatial Filters Based on Artifact and Brain Signal Topographies , 2002, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[34]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The design and analysis of state-trace experiments. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[35]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Mental Rotation: A Meta-analysis and Review , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  C. E. Bethell-Fox,et al.  Mental rotation: effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity , 1988 .

[37]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Non-identical neural mechanisms for two types of mental transformation: event-related potentials during mental rotation and mental paper folding , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[39]  R. Mayer,et al.  Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. , 2002 .

[40]  Carlo Miniussi,et al.  Parietal Lobe Contribution to Mental Rotation Demonstrated with rTMS , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: gender differences in growth patterns , 2008 .

[42]  Richard Henson,et al.  Forward inference using functional neuroimaging: dissociations versus associations , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.