Neurophysiological responses during cooking food associated with different emotions

Neurophysiological correlates of affective experience could potentially provide continuous information about a person’s experience when cooking and tasting food, without explicitly verbalizing this. Such measures would be helpful to understand people’s implicit food preferences and choices. This study examined for the first time the relation between neurophysiological variables and affective experiences under real cooking and tasting circumstances, using ingredients that were a priori expected to evoke different affective reactions. 41 participants cooked and tasted two stir-fry dishes in random order following an identical, strictly timed protocol. Once the main ingredient was chicken and the other time mealworms. EEG, ECG and skin potential were recorded continuously. Participants scored subjective valence and arousal after each cooking and tasting session. Frontal EEG alpha asymmetry showed the expected effect throughout the whole cooking and tasting session, consistent with ‘approach’ motivation for chicken and ‘avoidance’ for mealworms. Skin potential effects differed between cooking intervals but were in the expected direction. ECG variables showed an interaction with order of cooking the different dishes. Based on EEG alpha asymmetry, ECG and skin potential variables, we can estimate with 82% accuracy whether a single participant is preparing a dish with mealworms or with chicken. Our study provides evidence that it is possible to estimate experienced emotion during real-life cooking and tasting. We argue that it is important to consider that different neurophysiological and subjective measures reflect different underlying affective processes, to map them out more precisely, and to take advantage of these differences.

[1]  Florence V. Dunkel,et al.  How then shall we eat? Insect-eating attitudes and sustainable foodways , 2014 .

[2]  John L. Andreassi,et al.  Psychophysiology: Human Behavior & Physiological Response , 2000 .

[3]  Uta Noppeney,et al.  Music expertise shapes audiovisual temporal integration windows for speech, sinewave speech, and music , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[4]  José del R. Millán,et al.  BNCI Horizon 2020: Towards a Roadmap for the BCI Community , 2015 .

[5]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Introspecting about Reasons can Reduce Post-Choice Satisfaction , 1993 .

[6]  G. Berns,et al.  A Neural Predictor of Cultural Popularity , 2010 .

[7]  A. Fallon,et al.  A perspective on disgust. , 1987, Psychological review.

[8]  Alan L. Yen,et al.  Edible insects: Traditional knowledge or western phobia? , 2009 .

[9]  C. Graaf,et al.  Self-reported food-evoked emotions of younger adults, older normosmic adults, and older hyposmic adults as measured using the PrEmo2 tool and the Affect Grid , 2016 .

[10]  M. Ashton,et al.  The HEXACO–60: A Short Measure of the Major Dimensions of Personality , 2009, Journal of personality assessment.

[11]  P. Pliner,et al.  Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans , 1992, Appetite.

[12]  Pedro M. Domingos A few useful things to know about machine learning , 2012, Commun. ACM.

[13]  William H. Hampton,et al.  Predicting Advertising success beyond Traditional Measures: New Insights from Neurophysiological Methods and Market Response Modeling , 2015 .

[14]  G. Breithardt,et al.  Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. , 1996 .

[15]  Thomas Hummel,et al.  The sensory channel of presentation alters subjective ratings and autonomic responses toward disgusting stimuli—Blood pressure, heart rate and skin conductance in response to visual, auditory, haptic and olfactory presented disgusting stimuli , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  A. Steptoe,et al.  Positive affect and psychobiological processes , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[18]  P. Lang,et al.  Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. , 1989 .

[19]  Herbert L. Meiselman,et al.  Development of a method to measure consumer emotions associated with foods , 2010 .

[20]  P. Desmet,et al.  Introducing PrEmo2 : New directions for the non-verbal measurement of emotion in design , 2012 .

[21]  Clark McCauley,et al.  Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors , 1994 .

[22]  L.P.A. Steenbekkers,et al.  Insects as food: Exploring cultural exposure and individual experience as determinants of acceptance , 2015 .

[23]  Evaluation of research methods to study domestic food preparation , 2015 .

[24]  A. Malliani,et al.  Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use , 1996 .

[25]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[26]  M. Bradley,et al.  Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[27]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Neural Activity Underlying the Effect of Approach-Motivated Positive Affect on Narrowed Attention , 2009, Psychological science.

[28]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  The 'Trier Social Stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[29]  A. Cardello,et al.  Measuring emotional responses to foods and food names using questionnaires , 2012 .

[30]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Maarten A. Hogervorst,et al.  A new paradigm to induce mental stress: the Sing-a-Song Stress Test (SSST) , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[32]  Maarten A. Hogervorst,et al.  Physiological Correlates of Stress in Individuals about to Undergo Eye Laser Surgery , 2013, 2013 Humaine Association Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction.

[33]  Sylvia D. Kreibig,et al.  Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[34]  S. Boesveldt,et al.  The relation between continuous and discrete emotional responses to food odors with facial expressions and non-verbal reports , 2016 .

[35]  Maarten A. Hogervorst,et al.  Physiological signals distinguish between reading emotional and non-emotional sections in a novel , 2015 .

[36]  J. E. Korteling,et al.  Using neurophysiological signals that reflect cognitive or affective state: six recommendations to avoid common pitfalls , 2015, Front. Neurosci..

[37]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Relative left frontal activation to appetitive stimuli: considering the role of individual differences. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[38]  Emily B. Falk,et al.  From Neural Responses to Population Behavior , 2012, Psychological science.

[39]  Daniel Paech,et al.  Nuclear Overhauser Enhancement Mediated Chemical Exchange Saturation Transfer Imaging at 7 Tesla in Glioblastoma Patients , 2014, PloS one.

[40]  R. Leslie,et al.  Edible insects and associated food habits in Thailand. , 2010 .

[41]  M. Benedek,et al.  A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[42]  Christopher J Patrick,et al.  The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Maarten A. Hogervorst,et al.  Combining and comparing EEG, peripheral physiology and eye-related measures for the assessment of mental workload , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[44]  Radhika Bongoni,et al.  Studying consumer behaviour related to the quality of food: A case on vegetable preparation affecting sensory and health attributes , 2013 .

[45]  A. Lawrence,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  T. Gruber,et al.  Event-related frontal alpha asymmetries: electrophysiological correlates of approach motivation , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  P. Grossman,et al.  Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[48]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[49]  Fear imagery and the startle-probe reflex. , 1990 .

[50]  B. J. Ramos‐Elorduy The importance of edible insects in the nutrition and economy of people of the rural areas of Mexico , 1997 .

[51]  S. Boesveldt,et al.  Dynamics of autonomic nervous system responses and facial expressions to odors , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[52]  Sara R. Jaeger,et al.  Impact of individual differences in emotional intensity and private body consciousness on EsSense Profile® responses , 2013 .

[53]  H. Nagaraja,et al.  Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[54]  Cees de Graaf,et al.  Autonomic nervous system responses on and facial expressions to the sight, smell, and taste of liked and disliked foods , 2012 .

[55]  C. Ottaviani,et al.  Autonomic correlates of physical and moral disgust. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[56]  S. Uijtdehaage,et al.  Assessing the accuracy of topographic EEG mapping for determining local brain function. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  M. Bradley,et al.  Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. , 1994, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[58]  Jan B. F. van Erp,et al.  Evidence for effects of task difficulty but not learning on neurophysiological variables associated with effort. , 2014, International Journal of Psychophysiology.

[59]  J. Abramowitz,et al.  The Disgust Scale: item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. , 2007, Psychological assessment.

[60]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[61]  Dianne Neumark-Sztainer,et al.  Food preparation by young adults is associated with better diet quality. , 2006, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[62]  M. Mensink,et al.  ANS Responses and Facial Expressions Differentiate between the Taste of Commercial Breakfast Drinks , 2014, PloS one.

[63]  B. Piqueras-Fiszman,et al.  Sandwich or sweets? An assessment of two novel implicit association tasks to capture dynamic motivational tendencies and stable evaluations towards foods , 2016 .

[64]  W. Verbeke Profiling consumers who are ready to adopt insects as a meat substitute in a Western society , 2015 .

[65]  A. Damasio,et al.  Basic emotions are associated with distinct patterns of cardiorespiratory activity. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[66]  Klaus-Robert Müller,et al.  Introduction to machine learning for brain imaging , 2011, NeuroImage.

[67]  C. Spence,et al.  “Yummy” versus “Yucky”! Explicit and implicit approach–avoidance motivations towards appealing and disgusting foods , 2014, Appetite.

[68]  H. Lackner,et al.  Prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry changes while observing disaster happening to other people: Cardiac correlates and prediction of emotional impact , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[69]  P. Ekman,et al.  Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology. I. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.