Individual Differences in Route-Learning Strategy and Associated Working Memory Resources

Objective: The current investigation examined individual differences in route-learning strategies and their relative demands on visuospatial versus verbal working memory (WM) resources in virtual environments. Background: Learning new routes is a resource-demanding activity that must often be carried out in conjunction with other concurrent tasks. Virtual environments (VEs) are increasingly being used for training and research, pointing to the importance of determining the strategies people use to learn routes in these environments. Methods: Participants classified as having good or poor sense of direction (SOD) attempted to learn novel routes while concurrently performing either a verbal (articulatory suppression) or a visuospatial (tapping) WM interference task. Results: Different navigational strategies were observed in each SOD group. Individuals with poor SOD relied more heavily on verbal rather than visuospatial WM resources, as evidenced by greater disruption to route-learning performance from the articulatory suppression task relative to the tapping task. Conversely, individuals with good SOD exhibited more route-learning disruption from the tapping task, suggesting a greater reliance on visuospatial WM resources. Conclusion: Individuals differ from one another in the strategies they use and the WM resources they tap—verbal or visuospatial—to learn routes in VEs. Self-report measures can be used as indices of such individual differences in navigational strategy use in VE tasks. Application: Assessing SOD and associated WM resources have implications for targeted training for navigation in VEs and for the design of in-vehicle navigation systems.

[1]  C. Lawton,et al.  Gender Differences in Wayfinding Strategies and Anxiety About Wayfinding: A Cross-Cultural Comparison , 2002 .

[2]  C. Lawton Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety , 1994 .

[3]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. , 2002 .

[4]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[5]  Sharin E. Garden,et al.  Visuo-spatial working memory in navigation , 2002 .

[6]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Automation Reliability in Unmanned Aerial Vehicle Control: A Reliance-Compliance Model of Automation Dependence in High Workload , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[7]  Kay M. Stanney,et al.  A Theoretical Model of Wayfinding in Virtual Environments: Proposed Strategies for Navigational Aiding , 1999, Presence.

[8]  L. Gugerty,et al.  Reference-frame misalignment and cardinal direction judgments: group differences and strategies. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[9]  M. Hegarty,et al.  A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[10]  D. Saucier,et al.  Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. De Beni,et al.  Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals , 2001 .

[12]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Mission Control of Multiple Unmanned Aerial Vehicles: A Workload Analysis , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[13]  R. Shepard,et al.  The time required to prepare for a rotated stimulus , 1973, Memory & cognition.

[14]  Fabio Ferlazzo,et al.  Crossing the Pillars of Hercules: The Role of Spatial Frames of Reference in Error Making , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: An fMRI study of mental rotation , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  Sense of direction, spatial orientation, and cognitive maps. , 1977 .

[17]  C. Segebarth,et al.  Categorical and coordinate spatial relations: fMRI evidence for hemispheric specialization. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[18]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The effects of maps on navigation and search strategies in very-large-scale virtual environments , 1999 .

[19]  Sabrina Fagioli,et al.  Shifting attention across near and far spaces: implications for the use of hands-free cell phones while driving. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[20]  Steve Pettifer,et al.  Transfer of route learning from virtual to real environments. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[21]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[22]  Fred W. Mast,et al.  The neural basis of the egocentric and allocentric spatial frame of reference , 2007, Brain Research.

[23]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  When is early visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery? , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  F. Previc The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[26]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  The neural basis of ego- and allocentric reference frames in spatial navigation: Evidence from spatio-temporal coupled current density reconstruction , 2006, Brain Research.

[28]  C. Lawton STRATEGIES FOR INDOOR WAYFINDING: THE ROLE OF ORIENTATION , 1996 .

[29]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[30]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences , 2003 .

[31]  D. Saucier,et al.  Males and females scan maps similarly, but give directions differently , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  Navigation Training in Virtual Environments , 2001 .

[33]  J Y C Chen,et al.  Effects of tactile cueing on concurrent performance of military and robotics tasks in a simulated multitasking environment , 2008, Ergonomics.

[34]  Yoshiaki Takeuchi,et al.  SENSE OF DIRECTION AND ITS RELATIONSHIP WITH GEOGRAPHICAL ORIENTATION, PERSONALITY TRAITS AND MENTAL ABILITY , 1992 .

[35]  Michael A. Motes,et al.  Individual differences in the representations of novel environments , 2005 .

[36]  J. M. Dabbs,et al.  Spatial Ability, Navigation Strategy, and Geographic Knowledge Among Men and Women , 1998 .

[37]  Carryl L. Baldwin,et al.  Facilitating route memory with auditory route guidance systems. , 2006 .

[38]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Human Hippocampus and Spatial and Episodic Memory , 2002, Neuron.

[39]  K. Morrin,et al.  Gender Differences in Pointing Accuracy in Computer-Simulated 3D Mazes , 1999 .

[40]  P. Hancock,et al.  Human Mental Workload , 1988 .

[41]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning , 2006 .

[42]  C. Lawton,et al.  Gender and Regional Differences in Spatial Referents Used in Direction Giving , 2001 .

[43]  Doug A. Bowman,et al.  Maintaining Spatial Orientation during Travel in an Immersive Virtual Environment , 1999, Presence.

[44]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Neural representations in human spatial memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Miles C. Bowman,et al.  Sex differences in the effect of articulatory or spatial dual-task interference during navigation , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[46]  Nancy J. Cooke Human Factors of Remotely Operated Vehicles , 2006 .

[47]  Carryl L. Baldwin Individual differences in navigational strategy: implications for display design , 2009 .

[48]  Yoshiaki Takeuchi,et al.  Individual differences in wayfinding strategies , 2003 .