Generalization patterns for reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration differ after visuomotor learning.

Visuomotor learning results in changes in both motor and sensory systems (Cressman EK, Henriques DY. J Neurophysiol 102: 3505-3518, 2009), such that reaches are adapted and sense of felt hand position recalibrated after reaching with altered visual feedback of the hand. Moreover, visuomotor learning has been shown to generalize such that reach adaptation achieved at a trained target location can influence reaches to novel target directions (Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C. J Neurosci 20: 8916-8924, 2000). We looked to determine whether proprioceptive recalibration also generalizes to novel locations. Moreover, we looked to establish the relationship between reach adaptation and changes in sense of felt hand position by determining whether proprioceptive recalibration generalizes to novel targets in a similar manner as reach adaptation. On training trials, subjects reached to a single target with aligned or misaligned cursor-hand feedback, in which the cursor was either rotated or scaled in extent relative to hand movement. After reach training, subjects reached to the training target and novel targets (including targets from a second start position) without visual feedback to assess generalization of reach adaptation. Subjects then performed a proprioceptive estimation task, in which they indicated the position of their hand relative to visual reference markers placed at similar locations as the trained and novel reach targets. Results indicated that shifts in hand position generalized across novel locations, independent of reach adaptation. Thus these distinct sensory and motor generalization patterns suggest that reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration arise from independent error signals and that changes in one system cannot guide adjustments in the other.

[1]  D. Henriques,et al.  The effect of visuomotor adaptation on proprioceptive localization: the contributions of perceptual and motor changes , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Erin K. Cressman,et al.  Generalization of reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration at different distances in the workspace , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[5]  Peter Thier,et al.  Internalizing agency of self-action: perception of one's own hand movements depends on an adaptable prediction about the sensory action outcome. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Visual-shift adaptation is composed of separable sensory and task-dependent effects. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Erin K Cressman,et al.  Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Generalization to Local Remappings of the Visuomotor Coordinate Transformation , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  D. Henriques,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration in older adults , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  P. Thier,et al.  The Cerebellum Updates Predictions about the Visual Consequences of One's Behavior , 2008, Current Biology.

[11]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Cerebellar involvement in motor but not sensory adaptation , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  B. Treutwein Adaptive psychophysical procedures , 1995, Vision Research.

[13]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[14]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[15]  J. Krakauer,et al.  A computational neuroanatomy for motor control , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Learning not to generalize: modular adaptation of visuomotor gain. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Paolo Viviani,et al.  Altering the visuomotor gain. Evidence that motor plans deal with vector quantities. , 2002, Experimental brain research.

[18]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[19]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor learning and its sensory effects: time course of perceptual change and its presence with gradual introduction of load. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Ahmed A. Mostafa,et al.  Intermanual transfer and proprioceptive recalibration following training with translated visual feedback of the hand , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Erin K. Cressman,et al.  Proprioceptive localization of the left and right hands , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Somatosensory Plasticity and Motor Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  R. Miall,et al.  Evidence for stronger visuo-motor than visuo-proprioceptive conflict during mirror drawing performed by a deafferented subject and control subjects , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Bias and sensitivity in the haptic perception of geometry , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  C. S. Harris Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.

[28]  D. Henriques,et al.  Proprioceptive recalibration in the right and left hands following abrupt visuomotor adaptation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Spatially selective enhancement of proprioceptive acuity following motor learning. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  R. Miall,et al.  The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Erin K. Cressman,et al.  The role of the cross-sensory error signal in visuomotor adaptation , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an eye-centered spherical representation of the visuomotor map. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  D. Henriques,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation does not recalibrate kinesthetic sense of felt hand path. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Erin K Cressman,et al.  Visuomotor Adaptation and Proprioceptive Recalibration , 2012, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  D. Henriques,et al.  Proprioceptive recalibration following prolonged training and increasing distortions in visuomotor adaptation , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Rotations Remaps Movement Vectors, Not Final Positions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[41]  D. Henriques,et al.  Motor adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[42]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  T. Milner,et al.  Functionally Specific Changes in Resting-State Sensorimotor Networks after Motor Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  D. Henriques,et al.  Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[46]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Reach Adaptation and Learning Sensory Consequences of Action , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Proprioception plays a different role for sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions. , 2011, Human movement science.

[48]  Borís Burle,et al.  Direct evidence for cortical suppression of somatosensory afferents during visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  H. Kesten Accelerated Stochastic Approximation , 1958 .