Similar Effects of Feature-Based Attention on Motion Perception and Pursuit Eye Movements at Different Levels of Awareness

Feature-based attention enhances visual processing and improves perception, even for visual features that we are not aware of. Does feature-based attention also modulate motor behavior in response to visual information that does or does not reach awareness? Here we compare the effect of feature-based attention on motion perception and smooth-pursuit eye movements in response to moving dichoptic plaids—stimuli composed of two orthogonally drifting gratings, presented separately to each eye—in human observers. Monocular adaptation to one grating before the presentation of both gratings renders the adapted grating perceptually weaker than the unadapted grating and decreases the level of awareness. Feature-based attention was directed to either the adapted or the unadapted grating's motion direction or to both (neutral condition). We show that observers were better at detecting a speed change in the attended than the unattended motion direction, indicating that they had successfully attended to one grating. Speed change detection was also better when the change occurred in the unadapted than the adapted grating, indicating that the adapted grating was perceptually weaker. In neutral conditions, perception and pursuit in response to plaid motion were dissociated: While perception followed one grating's motion direction almost exclusively (component motion), the eyes tracked the average of both gratings (pattern motion). In attention conditions, perception and pursuit were shifted toward the attended component. These results suggest that attention affects perception and pursuit similarly even though only the former reflects awareness. The eyes can track an attended feature even if observers do not perceive it.

[1]  A. Kohn Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Keith A Schneider,et al.  Subcortical Mechanisms of Feature-Based Attention , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Attention without awareness in blindsight , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Scope and Limits of Top-Down Attention in Unconscious Visual Processing , 2006, Current Biology.

[5]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[6]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[7]  R J Krauzlis,et al.  Activation and inactivation of rostral superior colliculus neurons during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Dario L. Ringach,et al.  When your eyes see more than you do , 2010, Current Biology.

[9]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Subcortical Modulation of Attention Counters Change Blindness , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Signals Conveyed in the Pulvinar Pathway from Superior Colliculus to Cortical Area MT , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Laurent Itti,et al.  Perceptual consequences of feature-based attention. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[12]  Sean T. Stevens,et al.  Comparing the time course and efficacy of spatial and feature-based attention , 2007, Vision Research.

[13]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Vector Averaging for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Initiated by Two Moving Targets in Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Bisley The neural basis of visual attention , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[16]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[17]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Spatial allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[18]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Attention and target selection for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Amy A. Rezec,et al.  Attention enhances adaptability: evidence from motion adaptation experiments , 2004, Vision Research.

[20]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash , 1984, Vision Research.

[21]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Private lines of cortical visual information to the nucleus of the optic tract and dorsolateral pontine nucleus. , 2008, Progress in brain research.

[22]  Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al.  Feature‐based attention modulates direction‐selective hemodynamic activity within human MT , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[23]  R. Krauzlis The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[24]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Paradiso,et al.  Feature-specific effects of selective visual attention , 1995, Vision Research.

[26]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Detecting saccades with jerk , 1998, Vision Research.

[27]  Alex L. White,et al.  Feature-based attention involuntarily and simultaneously improves visual performance across locations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[28]  Theodore Raphan,et al.  Functions of the nucleus of the optic tract (NOT). , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Christopher D. Carello,et al.  Manipulating Intent Evidence for a Causal Role of the Superior Colliculus in Target Selection , 2004, Neuron.

[30]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Analysis of a naturally occurring asymmetry in vertical smooth pursuit eye movements in a monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  G. Boynton,et al.  Feature-Based Attentional Modulations in the Absence of Direct Visual Stimulation , 2007, Neuron.

[32]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contrast and assimilation in motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  W T Newsome,et al.  How Is a Sensory Map Read Out? Effects of Microstimulation in Visual Area MT on Saccades and Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Blake,et al.  Neural strength of visual attention gauged by motion adaptation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Feature-Based Attention Modulates Orientation-Selective Responses in Human Visual Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[37]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  A. Montagnini,et al.  Do we track what we see? Common versus independent processing for motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements: A review , 2011, Vision Research.

[39]  Stephen G Lisberger,et al.  Visual Guidance of Smooth-Pursuit Eye Movements: Sensation, Action, and What Happens in Between , 2010, Neuron.

[40]  F. Hamker,et al.  Attention Alters Feature Space in Motion Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.

[43]  G. Boynton,et al.  Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Najib J Majaj,et al.  Binocular Integration of Pattern Motion Signals by MT Neurons and by Human Observers , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Distractor interference during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[47]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[48]  P. Schiller,et al.  Discharge characteristics of single units in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  T. Isa,et al.  Saccade control after V1 lesion revisited , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  D. Melcher,et al.  Implicit Attentional Selection of Bound Visual Features , 2005, Neuron.

[51]  Janita Turchi,et al.  Pulvinar Inactivation Disrupts Selection of Movement Plans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Béatrice de Gelder,et al.  Collicular Vision Guides Nonconscious Behavior , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[54]  Lotfi B. Merabet,et al.  Motion integration in a thalamic visual nucleus , 1998, Nature.

[55]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Perceptual Relearning of Complex Visual Motion after V1 Damage in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Functional Identification of a Pulvinar Path from Superior Colliculus to Cortical Area MT , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  M. Carrasco,et al.  How do attention and adaptation affect contrast sensitivity? , 2007, Journal of vision.

[58]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  When sustained attention impairs perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[59]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[60]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Feature-based attention in visual cortex , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[61]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Learning to reach for ‘invisible’ visual input , 2011, Current Biology.

[62]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Tracking Without Perceiving , 2011, Psychology Science.

[63]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Thalamic pathways for active vision , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  Brian H Scott,et al.  A comparison of pursuit eye movement and perceptual performance in speed discrimination. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[65]  Daryl E. Wilson,et al.  Control of Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in Frontoparietal Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Superior Colliculus Inactivation Alters the Weighted Integration of Visual Stimuli , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Contrast gain control in the cat visual cortex , 1982, Nature.

[68]  Christof Koch,et al.  Attention and consciousness: related yet different , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  D. Leopold,et al.  Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  Thomas Schmidt,et al.  Feature-based attention to unconscious shapes and colors , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[71]  Stephen G Lisberger,et al.  Directional Cuing of Target Choice in Human Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Shift in smooth pursuit initiation and MT and MST neuronal activity under different stimulus conditions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  M. Carrasco,et al.  How spatial and feature-based attention affect the gain and tuning of population responses , 2009, Vision Research.

[74]  J. Todd,et al.  The effects of viewing angle, camera angle, and sign of surface curvature on the perception of three-dimensional shape from texture. , 2007, Journal of vision.