Language and vertical space: On the automaticity of language action interconnections
暂无分享,去创建一个
Barbara Kaup | Irmgard de la Vega | Carolin Dudschig | Monica De Filippis | Carolin Dudschig | M. Filippis | I. Vega | Barbara Kaup
[1] Amy Needham,et al. Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infants , 1993, Cognition.
[2] S. Dehaene,et al. A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[3] Barbara Kaup,et al. Reading “Sun” and Looking Up: The Influence of Language on Saccadic Eye Movements in the Vertical Dimension , 2013, PloS one.
[4] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[5] Barbara Kaup,et al. When up-words meet down-sentences: evidence for word- or sentence-based compatibility effects? , 2012, Cognitive Processing.
[6] D. Lakens. High Skies and Oceans Deep: Polarity Benefits or Mental Simulation? , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[7] Srini Narayanan,et al. Spatial and Linguistic Aspects of Visual Imagery in Sentence Comprehension , 2007, Cogn. Sci..
[8] J. Hoffmann,et al. Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.
[9] Jijia Zhang,et al. Neural mechanisms of shifts of spatial attention induced by object words with spatial associations: an ERP study , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[10] M. Hautus. Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values ofd′ , 1995 .
[11] I. Johnsrude,et al. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.
[12] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. Putting words in perspective , 2004, Memory & cognition.
[14] P. Haggard,et al. Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.
[15] E. Bernat,et al. Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[16] M. Eimer. Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[17] Barbara Kaup,et al. Embodiment and second-language: Automatic activation of motor responses during processing spatially associated L2 words and emotion L2 words in a vertical Stroop paradigm , 2014, Brain and Language.
[18] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[19] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] Sean Duffy,et al. Spatial Representations Elicit Dual-Coding Effects in Mental Imagery , 2009, Cogn. Sci..
[21] Barbara Kaup,et al. Do task-irrelevant direction-associated motion verbs affect action planning? Evidence from a Stroop paradigm , 2012, Memory & cognition.
[22] C. Umilta,et al. Simon effect with and without awareness of the accessory stimulus. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] Stephanie Ortigue,et al. The Neural Basis of Love as a Subliminal Prime: An Event-related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] L. Barsalou,et al. Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[25] B. Kaup,et al. Root versus roof: automatic activation of location information during word processing , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[26] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Embodied Language: A Review of the Role of the Motor System in Language Comprehension , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[27] George S. Cree,et al. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.
[28] N A Macmillan,et al. Detection theory analysis of group data: estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[29] E. Maylor,et al. No difference between conscious and nonconscious visuomotor control: Evidence from perceptual learning in the masked prime task , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[30] Michael E R Nicholls,et al. The highs and lows of the interaction between word meaning and space. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] S. Dehaene,et al. The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[32] P. König,et al. Space-Valence Priming with Subliminal and Supraliminal Words , 2013, Front. Psychology.
[33] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Motor Resonance and Linguistic Focus , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[34] S. Dehaene,et al. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.
[35] A. Glenberg,et al. Processing Abstract Language Modulates Motor System Activity , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[36] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[37] J. Pratt,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General The Cost and Benefit of Implicit Spatial Cues for Visual Attention , 2012 .
[38] L. Vainio,et al. Motor inhibition associated with the affordance of briefly displayed objects , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[39] D. Lakens. Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[40] L. Barsalou,et al. Head Up, Foot Down , 2008, Psychological science.
[41] Terhi Mustonen,et al. Mapping the identity of a viewed hand in the motor system: evidence from stimulus-response compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. The Action-Sentence Compatibility Effect: It's All in the Timing , 2006, Cogn. Sci..
[43] J B Poline,et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[44] M. Coles. Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[45] P. König,et al. Testing the theory of embodied cognition with subliminal words , 2010, Cognition.
[46] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Embodied Sentence Comprehension , 2005 .
[47] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[48] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[49] A. Borghi,et al. Sentence comprehension and action: Effector specific modulation of the motor system , 2007, Brain Research.
[50] Derek Besner,et al. The stroop effect and the myth of automaticity , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[51] M. Bowerman. The origins of children's spatial semantic categories: Cognitive vs. linguistic determinants , 1996 .
[52] Michele T. Diaz,et al. Unconscious Word Processing Engages a Distributed Network of Brain Regions , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[53] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[54] W. Brewer,et al. Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[55] S. Klapp,et al. The negative compatibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection , 2002 .
[56] L. Mulder,et al. Use of partial stimulus information in response processing. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] A. Glenberg,et al. Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production , 2012, Cortex.
[58] S. Levinson. Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity , 2003 .
[59] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[60] M. Eimer,et al. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] Mark Johnson,et al. The Metaphorical Structure of the Human Conceptual System , 1980, Cogn. Sci..
[62] Barbara Kaup,et al. From top to bottom: spatial shifts of attention caused by linguistic stimuli , 2012, Cognitive Processing.
[63] H. Kornhuber,et al. Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.
[64] M. Jeannerod,et al. Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[65] M. Eimer,et al. Locus of Inhibition in the Masked Priming of Response Alternatives , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[66] M. Eimer,et al. Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.