Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory

476 NATURE | VOL 411 | 24 MAY 2001 | www.nature.com about the test grating (48 c.p.d.) on the frequency scale. The contrast of the 36 c.p.d. adapting grating was set to half of its threshold contrast, 3.5%. No measurable tilt after-effect was found under this condition (Fig. 2b). Similarly, in measurements of the elevation in contrast threshold produced by pre-exposure to a range of adapting grating contrasts, at three spatial frequencies spanning the resolution limit, we found that adapting gratings of frequency greater than the resolution limit were more effective than correspondingly subthreshold gratings of lower frequency (although subthreshold gratings in the high but resolvable frequency range were not entirely ineffective). Thus, the cortical requirement for conscious perception seems to depend on spatial frequency, and not only on contrast or signal strength represented at the cortical input. The idea that limits on visual resolution are partly imposed at the cortical level is supported by evidence that cortically projecting thalamic relay neurons in macaque often respond well to spatial frequencies far above the human resolution limit, in some cases exceeding 100 c.p.d. (ref. 15). If the projection from thalamus to cortex were as precise as the one from retina to thalamus, this would allow the visual system to form a representation of unresolvable patterns at the cortical site of pattern adaptation. The lower spatialfrequency limits for cortical after-effects (70 c.p.d.), as compared with thalamic neurons (100 c.p.d.), may re ̄ect imperfect precision in the projection from thalamus to cortex. In normal vision with incoherent light, diffraction markedly reduces the retinal image contrast for spatial frequencies near the resolution limit. Why should the cortex have orientation-selective mechanisms (or frequency-selective ones) that respond to high spatial frequencies that are normally only faintly represented in the retinal image? One answer derives from the view that neural mechanisms might compensate for optical blur: frequency components that have been optically attenuated might even require a reciprocal enhancement of neural sensitivity for their appropriate representation. But on our evidence, activation of orientation-selective units at the stage of cortical pattern adaptation is not suf®cient for perceptual awareness of the pattern orientation. The nature of the added requirement is not clear: one possibility is that information must be relayed from primary visual cortex to another region of the brain to be represented in conscious experience. M

[1]  H. Barlow,et al.  Single Units and Sensation: A Neuron Doctrine for Perceptual Psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[2]  J. Gibson,et al.  ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .

[3]  M. Low,et al.  Leptin activates anorexigenic POMC neurons through a neural network in the arcuate nucleus , 2001, Nature.

[4]  J. Dubnau,et al.  Functional anatomy: From molecule to memory , 2001, Current Biology.

[5]  W. Quinn,et al.  The amnesiac Gene Product Is Expressed in Two Neurons in the Drosophila Brain that Are Critical for Memory , 2000, Cell.

[6]  E. Shimizu,et al.  NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic reinforcement as a crucial process for memory consolidation. , 2000, Science.

[7]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. , 2000, Science.

[8]  D. Linden,et al.  Expression of Cerebellar Long-Term Depression Requires Postsynaptic Clathrin-Mediated Endocytosis , 2000, Neuron.

[9]  P. Lennie,et al.  Fine Structure of Parvocellular Receptive Fields in the Primate Fovea Revealed by Laser Interferometry , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Nicoll,et al.  Dynamin-dependent endocytosis of ionotropic glutamate receptors. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Organization of olfactory and multimodal afferent neurons supplying the calyx and pedunculus of the cockroach mushroom bodies , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[12]  Leslie C. Griffith,et al.  Mapping of the anatomical circuit of CaM kinase-dependent courtship conditioning in Drosophila. , 1999, Learning & memory.

[13]  M. Ramaswami,et al.  Probable mechanisms underlying interallelic complementation and temperature-sensitivity of mutations at the shibire locus of Drosophila melanogaster. , 1998, Genetics.

[14]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  The organization of extrinsic neurons and their implications in the functional roles of the mushroom bodies in Drosophila melanogaster Meigen. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[15]  M. Hammer,et al.  Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[16]  J. Movshon,et al.  Pattern adaptation and cross-orientation interactions in the primary visual cortex , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[17]  E. Kandel,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory , 1998, Neuron.

[18]  Tim Tully,et al.  Associative Learning Disrupted by Impaired Gs Signaling in Drosophila Mushroom Bodies , 1996, Science.

[19]  E. Kandel,et al.  Control of Memory Formation Through Regulated Expression of a CaMKII Transgene , 1996, Science.

[20]  D. Macleod,et al.  Local luminance nonlinearity and receptor aliasing in the detection of high-frequency gratings. , 1996, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[21]  H S Smallman,et al.  Fine grain of the neural representation of human spatial vision , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Reid,et al.  Specificity of monosynaptic connections from thalamus to visual cortex , 1995, Nature.

[23]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Subdivision of the drosophila mushroom bodies by enhancer-trap expression patterns , 1995, Neuron.

[24]  C. Koch,et al.  Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? , 1995, Nature.

[25]  S. Schmid,et al.  Induction of mutant dynamin specifically blocks endocytic coated vesicle formation , 1994, The Journal of cell biology.

[26]  T. Préat,et al.  Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.

[27]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. , 1994, Science.

[28]  R. Kelly,et al.  Genetic studies on dynamin function in Drosophila. , 1993, Journal of neurogenetics.

[29]  D. Brainard,et al.  Efficiency in detection of isoluminant and isochromatic interference fringes. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[30]  R. Menzel,et al.  Anatomy of the mushroom bodies in the honey bee brain: The neuronal connections of the alpha‐lobe , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  N. Perrimon,et al.  Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. , 1993, Development.

[32]  Richard B. Vallee,et al.  Multiple forms of dynamin are encoded by shibire, a Drosophila gene involved in endocytosis , 1991, Nature.

[33]  Alexander M. van der Bliek,et al.  Dynamin-like protein encoded by the Drosophila shibire gene associated with vesicular traffic , 1991, Nature.

[34]  K. Ikeda,et al.  Disappearance and reformation of synaptic vesicle membrane upon transmitter release observed under reversible blockage of membrane retrieval , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Short test flashes produce large tilt aftereffects , 1984, Vision Research.

[36]  D M Levi,et al.  Humans deprived of normal binocular vision have binocular interactions tuned to size and orientation. , 1979, Science.

[37]  L. Edgar,et al.  Reversible alteration in the neuromuscular junctions of Drosophila melanogaster bearing a temperature-sensitive mutation, shibire , 1979, The Journal of cell biology.

[38]  P. Lennie,et al.  Pattern-selective adaptation in visual cortical neurones , 1979, Nature.

[39]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1977 .

[40]  L. Maffei,et al.  Neural Correlate of Perceptual Adaptation to Gratings , 1973, Science.

[41]  D. Suzuki,et al.  Developmental properties of Shibire: a pleiotropic mutation affecting larval and adult locomotion and development. , 1973, Developmental biology.

[42]  H B Barlow,et al.  Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[43]  M. Coltheart Visual feature-analyzers and after-effects of tilt and curvature. , 1971, Psychological review.

[44]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Adaptation to spatial stimuli. , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[45]  S. Benzer BEHAVIORAL MUTANTS OF Drosophila ISOLATED BY COUNTERCURRENT DISTRIBUTION. , 1967, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  F. Campbell,et al.  Optical quality of the human eye , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[48]  K. Abromeit Music Received , 2023, Notes.