Does cognitive distraction improve or degrade lane keeping performance? Analysis of time-to-line crossing safety margins

Abstract Studies on the effect of cognitive load (CL) on driving performance suggest that lane keeping performance is improved by cognitive distraction, due to a reduction in measures of the standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP). However, the effect of CL on drivers’ lateral control is still not fully understood, and previous studies have shown mixed conclusions regarding the effect of CL on time-to-line crossing (TLC) safety margins. Hence, a driving simulator experiment was performed, requiring performance an auditory-response working memory task (CL task), during driving, presented at of three difficulty levels. Similar to previous studies, CL led to increased micro-steering activity, as well as a diminished SDLP, implying a better lane keeping performance. However, a systematic comparison of TLC calculations showed that the TLC values consistently decreased with the CL task, suggesting a degraded safety margin of lane keeping. While these decreased TLCs did not bring the vehicle close to actual lane departure, they do put into question the general finding that lane keeping is improved by cognitive distraction. We discuss how the increased micro-steering activity could lead to the somewhat counterintuitive simultaneous decrease in both SDLP and TLC. In addition, we suggest the use of a new method for TLC calculations, assuming constant lateral acceleration. We argue that by involving short time windows (3–5 s) of chunking, this method may be useful for assessing drivers’ safety behavior, and correct detection of unsafe cognitive distraction.

[1]  J. Dusek,et al.  Impact of Incremental Increases in Cognitive Workload on Physiological Arousal and Performance in Young Adult Drivers , 2009 .

[2]  H Summala,et al.  Cognitive load and detection thresholds in car following situations: safety implications for using mobile (cellular) telephones while driving. , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[3]  Paul Milgram,et al.  The Development of a Time-Related Measure to Describe Driving Strategy , 1984 .

[4]  N Merat,et al.  Leading to distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment. , 2016, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[5]  Mark Mulder,et al.  The value of haptic feedback in lane keeping , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC).

[6]  David L. Strayer,et al.  The Impact of Eye Movements and Cognitive Workload on Lateral Position Variability in Driving , 2013, Hum. Factors.

[7]  Wendy Macdonald,et al.  Review of Relationships Between Steering Wheel Reversal Rate and Driving Task Demand , 1980 .

[8]  Barbara Metz,et al.  Distribution of visual attention during distraction: influence of demands of the driving task and of the secondary task , 2011 .

[9]  Yulan Liang,et al.  Driver performance effects of simultaneous visual and cognitive distraction and adaptation behavior , 2012 .

[10]  Bryan Reimer,et al.  Glance analysis of driver eye movements to evaluate distraction , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[11]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Effects of Cognitive Distraction on Lane-keeping , 2011 .

[12]  Ying Wang,et al.  The sensitivity of different methodologies for characterizing drivers’ gaze concentration under increased cognitive demand , 2014, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour.

[13]  John D Lee,et al.  Combining cognitive and visual distraction: less than the sum of its parts. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[14]  Simon Washington,et al.  The impact of mobile phone distraction on the braking behaviour of young drivers : a hazard-based duration model , 2014 .

[15]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Identifying cognitive distraction using steering wheel reversal rates. , 2016, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[16]  S. Glaser,et al.  Experimental time to line crossing validation , 2005, Proceedings. 2005 IEEE Intelligent Transportation Systems, 2005..

[17]  G Marrkula,et al.  A STEERING WHEEL REVERSAL RATE METRIC FOR ASSESSING EFFECTS OF VISUAL AND COGNITIVE SECONDARY TASK LOAD , 2006 .

[18]  H Godthelp,et al.  LEVELS OF STEERING CONTROL; SOME NOTES ON THE TIME-TO-LINE CROSSING CONCEPT AS RELATED TO DRIVING STRATEGY , 1981 .

[19]  Birsen Donmez,et al.  EMG provides an earlier glimpse into the effects of cognitive distraction on brake motor response , 2014 .

[20]  Sukhan Lee,et al.  A vision based lane departure warning system , 1999, Proceedings 1999 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems. Human and Environment Friendly Robots with High Intelligence and Emotional Quotients (Cat. No.99CH36289).

[21]  Bryan Reimer,et al.  Impact of Cognitive Task Complexity on Drivers’ Visual Tunneling , 2009 .

[22]  Simon Washington,et al.  A parametric duration model of the reaction times of drivers distracted by mobile phone conversations. , 2014, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[23]  Johan Engström,et al.  Sensitivity of eye-movement measures to in-vehicle task difficulty , 2005 .

[24]  Saïd Mammar,et al.  Time to line crossing for lane departure avoidance: a theoretical study and an experimental setting , 2006, IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems.

[25]  Thomas A. Dingus,et al.  The Impact of Driver Inattention on Near-Crash/Crash Risk: An Analysis Using the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Data , 2006 .

[26]  T. Victor Keeping Eye and Mind on the Road , 2005 .

[27]  Moshe Eizenman,et al.  THE IMPACT OF COGNITIVE DISTRACTION ON DRIVER VISUAL BEHAVIOUR AND VEHICLE CONTROL , 2002 .

[28]  K A Brookhuis,et al.  A comparison of different ways to approximate time-to-line crossing (TLC) during car driving. , 2000, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[29]  S. Glaser,et al.  Time-to-line crossing and vehicle dynamics for lane departure avoidance , 2004, Proceedings. The 7th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (IEEE Cat. No.04TH8749).

[30]  Frank Drews,et al.  Profiles in Driver Distraction: Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on Younger and Older Drivers , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[31]  Michelle L. Reyes,et al.  Effects of cognitive load presence and duration on driver eye movements and event detection performance , 2008 .

[32]  M. Friedman The Use of Ranks to Avoid the Assumption of Normality Implicit in the Analysis of Variance , 1937 .

[33]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Effects of Cognitive Load on Driving Performance: The Cognitive Control Hypothesis , 2017, Hum. Factors.

[34]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[35]  W. V. Winsum,et al.  Lane change manoeuvres and safety margins , 1999 .

[36]  D. Strayer,et al.  Provided for Non-commercial Research and Educational Use Only. Not for Reproduction, Distribution or Commercial Use. Cognitive Distraction While Multitasking in the Automobile , 2022 .

[37]  Geert Wets,et al.  Investigating the influence of working memory capacity when driving behavior is combined with cognitive load: An LCT study of young novice drivers. , 2014, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[38]  Johan Engström,et al.  Effects of visual and cognitive load in real and simulated motorway driving , 2005 .

[39]  Paul Green Why Driving Performance Measures Are Sometimes Not Accurate (and Methods to Check Accuracy) , 2017 .

[40]  Bryan Reimer,et al.  Sensitivity of Physiological Measures for Detecting Systematic Variations in Cognitive Demand From a Working Memory Task , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[41]  Joonwoo Son,et al.  Impact of traffic environment and cognitive workload on older drivers’ behavior in simulated driving , 2011 .

[42]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Surrogate in-vehicle information systems and driver behaviour: effects of visual and cognitive load in simulated rural driving , 2005 .

[43]  Mark Vollrath,et al.  The effect of visual and cognitive distraction on driver’s anticipation in a simulated car following scenario , 2011 .

[44]  Karel Brookhuis,et al.  Issues arising from the HASTE experiments , 2005 .

[45]  Louis Tijerina,et al.  Driver Workload Metrics Task 2 Final Report , 2006 .

[46]  Natasha Merat,et al.  The effect of a simulated hearing loss on performance of an auditory memory task in driving , 2016 .

[47]  John T. E. Richardson,et al.  Eta Squared and Partial Eta Squared as Measures of Effect Size in Educational Research. , 2011 .

[48]  Myra Blanco,et al.  The impact of secondary task cognitive processing demand on driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[49]  Thomas J Triggs,et al.  Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[50]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Lane Keeping Under Cognitive Load , 2014, Hum. Factors.

[51]  Michael A. Goodrich,et al.  Steering Entropy Revisited , 2017 .

[52]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Cognitive Driver Distraction Improves Straight Lane Keeping: A Cybernetic Control Theoretic Explanation , 2016 .

[53]  Marco Dozza,et al.  Chunking: a procedure to improve naturalistic data analysis. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[54]  Karel Brookhuis,et al.  HMI and Safety-Related Driver Performance , 2004 .