The role of the agranular insular cortex in working memory for food reward value and allocentric space in rats

The present experiment examined the effects of quinolinic acid (125 mM) lesions of the agranular insular area on working memory for food reward value and working memory for spatial locations. In both tasks a go/no-go procedure was used. Working memory for food reward value was assessed using a delayed conditional discrimination in which either a 20% or 45% sugar content cereal was associated with a reinforcement and the other cereal was not. In the spatial locations task, rats were allowed to enter 12 arms in a radial maze for a food reinforcement. Of the 12 arm presentations, three or four arms were presented for a second time in a session which did not contain a reinforcement. The number of trials between the 1st and 2nd presentation of an arm ranged from 0 to 6 (lags). Working memory was assessed by the latency to enter an arm during the 2nd presentation. In the food reward value task, agranular insular lesions produced memory deficits in a delay-dependent manner. In contrast, agranular insular lesions did not impair working memory for spatial locations. These results add to accumulating evidence suggesting that different types of working memory are distributed across separate prefrontal subregions.

[1]  R P Kesner,et al.  Differential involvement of the dorsal anterior cingulate and prelimbic-infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex in spatial working memory. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[2]  R. Sutherland,et al.  A comparison of the contributions of the frontal and parietal association cortex to spatial localization in rats. , 1983, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  R. Kesner,et al.  The Effects of Muscarinic Cholinergic Receptor Blockade in the Rat Anterior Cingulate and Prelimbic/Infralimbic Cortices on Spatial Working Memory , 1998, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[4]  Jeremy K. Seamans,et al.  Functional differences between the prelimbic and anterior cingulate regions of the rat prefrontal cortex. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  J. D. Bruin,et al.  Spatial delayed alternation of rats in a T-maze: effects of neurotoxic lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex and of T-maze rotations , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[6]  John Garcia,et al.  Involvement of the anterior insular gustatory neocortex in taste-potentiated odor aversion learning , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  J. D. Brabander,et al.  Response learning of rats in a Morris water maze: Involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  H. Groenewegen Organization of the afferent connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal-prefrontal topography , 1988, Neuroscience.

[9]  I. Divac,et al.  Selective ablations within the prefrontal cortex of the rat and performance of delayed alternation , 1978 .

[10]  B. Kolb,et al.  Comparisons of behavioral effects of hippocampal and prefrontal cortex lesions in the rat. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[11]  J. King,et al.  Hedonic reactivity to sucrose in rats: Modification by pimozide , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[12]  Howard Eichenbaum,et al.  Reexamination of functional subdivisions of the rodent prefrontal cortex , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[13]  N. White,et al.  Contingent and non-contingent actions of sucrose and saccharin reinforcers: Effects on taste preference and memory , 1984, Physiology & Behavior.

[14]  A. Thierry,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.

[15]  H. Groenewegen,et al.  Anatomical Relationships Between the Prefrontal Cortex and the Basal Ganglia in the Rat , 1994 .

[16]  P. Shizgal,et al.  Differential effects of postingestive feedback on the reward value of sucrose and lateral hypothalamic stimulation in rats. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  R. Roth,et al.  Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: An anterograde tract‐tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  D. van der Kooy,et al.  Visceral cortex: Integration of the mucosal senses with limbic information in the rat agranular insular cortex , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  R. Kesner,et al.  Short-term memory for food reward magnitude: The role of the prefrontal cortex , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  F. Bermúdez-Rattoni,et al.  Reversible inactivation of the insular cortex by tetrodotoxin produces retrograde and anterograde amnesia for inhibitory avoidance and spatial learning. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  James L. McGaugh,et al.  Insular cortex and amygdala lesions differentially affect acquisition on inhibitory avoidance and conditioned taste aversion , 1991, Brain Research.

[22]  R. Kesner,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and working memory for spatial response, spatial location, and visual object information in the rat. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  G. Paxinos,et al.  The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 1983 .

[24]  H. Uylings,et al.  Qualitative and quantitative comparison of the prefrontal cortex in rat and in primates, including humans. , 1990, Progress in brain research.