Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: manual and spatial asymmetries
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jon Driver,et al. Inhibition of return is supramodal: a demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] D A Rosenbaum,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in the programming of speech , 1987, Memory & cognition.
[3] M. Bryden. Measuring handedness with questionnaires , 1977, Neuropsychologia.
[4] K. Heilman,et al. Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.
[5] R. Cohen. The Neuropsychology of Attention , 1993, Critical Issues in Neuropsychology.
[6] Carlo Alberto Marzi,et al. Neuropsychology of Attention , 2020, Handbook of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology.
[7] R. Schmidt. Control Processes In Motor Skills , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.
[8] D. Elliott,et al. Hand deviations toward distractors Evidence for response competition , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] D. Carey,et al. Hemispatial differences in visually guided aiming are neither hemispatial nor visual , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[10] E. Maylor,et al. Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] D. Elliott,et al. Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .
[12] M. Posner,et al. Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .
[13] S. Tipper,et al. Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] S. Tipper,et al. Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Exploring the consequences of the previous trial , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[16] S. Tipper,et al. Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[18] R. Sainburg. Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] J. Cullen,et al. Manual Asymmetries in Goal-Directed Movement: Examination of the Motor Output Hypothesis , 1998, Brain and Cognition.
[20] A. Sereno,et al. Inhibition of return in manual and saccadic response systems , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[21] Steven P. Tipper,et al. Visual search and target-directed action , 1999 .
[22] S. Tipper,et al. Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[23] J. Hellige,et al. Hemispheric asymmetries in vigilance and cerebral arousal mechanisms in younger and older adults. , 1999, Neuropsychology.
[24] Digby Elliott,et al. Manual Asymmetries in Motor Performance , 1996 .
[25] S. Tipper,et al. Inhibition of return in a selective reaching task: an investigation of reference frames. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.
[26] Richard G. Carson,et al. Manual Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Goal-Directed Movements , 2001, Brain and Cognition.
[27] M. Raibert. Motor Control and Learning by the State Space Model , 1977 .
[28] D. Rosenbaum. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[29] D. Elliott,et al. Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1993 .
[30] S. Tipper,et al. Behavioral consequences of selection from neural population codes. , 2000 .
[31] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control , 1992 .
[32] G. Tassinari,et al. Ipsilateral inhibition and contralateral facilitation of simple reaction time to non-foveal visual targets from non-informative visual cues. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[33] B. Weaver,et al. The effects of practice on object-based, location-based, and static-display inhibition of return , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Space and selective attention , 1994 .
[35] J Driver,et al. Auditory and audiovisual inhibition of return , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] G. Tassinari,et al. The contribution of general and specific motor inhibitory sets to the so-called auditory inhibition of return , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] Jacob Cohen. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[38] S. Tipper,et al. Scene-based and object-centered inhibition of return: Evidence for dual orienting mechanisms , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] D. Elliott,et al. Movement Trajectories in the Presence of a Distracting Stimulus: Evidence for a Response Activation Model of Selective Reaching , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[40] Timothy N Welsh,et al. Effects of response priming and inhibition on movement planning and execution. , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.
[41] R. Abrams,et al. Inhibition of return: effects of attentional cuing on eye movement latencies. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] Steven P. Tipper,et al. Action-centred negative priming: Evidence for reactive inhibition , 2002 .