Causal saliency effects during natural vision

Salient stimuli, such as color or motion contrasts, attract human attention, thus providing a fast heuristic for focusing limited neural resources on behaviorally relevant sensory inputs. Here we address the following questions: What types of saliency attract attention and how do they compare to each other during natural vision? We asked human participants to inspect scene-shuffled video clips, tracked their instantaneous eye-position, and quantified how well a battery of computational saliency models predicted overt attentional selections (saccades). Saliency effects were measured as a function of total viewing time, proximity to abrupt scene transitions (jump cuts), and inter-participant consistency. All saliency models predicted overall attentional selection well above chance, with dynamic models being equally predictive to each other, and up to 3.6 times more predictive than static models. The prediction accuracy of all dynamic models was twice higher than their average for saccades that were initiated immediately after jump cuts, and led to maximal inter-participant consistency. Static models showed mixed results in these circumstances, with some models having weaker prediction accuracy than their average. These results demonstrate that dynamic visual cues play a dominant causal role in attracting attention, while static visual cues correlate with attentional selection mostly due to top-down causes.

[1]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[2]  R. Hernández-Peón,et al.  Modification of electric activity in cochlear nucleus during attention in unanesthetized cats. , 1956, Science.

[3]  Prof. Dr. Eberhard Curio The Ethology of Predation , 1976, Zoophysiology and Ecology.

[4]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[5]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Single visual neurons code opposing motion independent of direction. , 1983, Science.

[7]  D. Finlay,et al.  Cardiac and visual responses to moving stimuli presented either successively or simultaneously to the central and peripheral visual fields in 4-month-old infants. , 1984 .

[8]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  The representation of the visual field in parvicellular and magnocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus in the macaque monkey , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[9]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[10]  Julian Hochberg,et al.  Representation of motion and space in video and cinematic displays , 1986 .

[11]  A. Hendrickson,et al.  Distribution of cones in human and monkey retina: individual variability and radial asymmetry. , 1987, Science.

[12]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[14]  P. McLeod,et al.  Motion coherence and conjunction search: Implications for guided search theory , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 1994 .

[17]  U Polat,et al.  Spatial interactions in human vision: from near to far via experience-dependent cascades of connections. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual motion and attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  David Bordwell,et al.  Film History: An Introduction , 1994 .

[20]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  S. T. Buckland,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap. , 1994 .

[22]  G. Sperling,et al.  Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial attention. , 1995 .

[23]  H. Jones,et al.  Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities , 1995, Nature.

[24]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[25]  Joseph D. Anderson The reality of illusion : an ecological approach to cognitive film theory , 1996 .

[26]  D. S. Wooding,et al.  Fixation Patterns Made during Brief Examination of Two-Dimensional Images , 1997, Perception.

[27]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[28]  J. Henderson,et al.  High-level scene perception. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[29]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[30]  P Reinagel,et al.  Natural scene statistics at the centre of gaze. , 1999, Network.

[31]  J. Cutting The Reality of Illusion: An Ecological Approach to Cognitive Film Theory , 1999 .

[32]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Michael F. Land,et al.  From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Attention is fast but volition is slow , 2000, Nature.

[35]  G. Hauske,et al.  Object and scene analysis by saccadic eye-movements: an investigation with higher-order statistics. , 2000, Spatial vision.

[36]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[37]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Visual scene memory and the guidance of saccadic eye movements , 2001, Vision Research.

[38]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Noticing Familiar Objects in Real World Scenes: The Role of Temporal Cortical Neurons in Natural Vision , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  J. Mollon,et al.  Fruits, foliage and the evolution of primate colour vision. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[41]  R. Jacobs What determines visual cue reliability? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  P. Bex,et al.  Spatial frequency, phase, and the contrast of natural images. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[43]  Derrick J. Parkhurst,et al.  Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention , 2002, Vision Research.

[44]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Insect behaviour: Motion camouflage in dragonflies , 2003, Nature.

[46]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Top-down control of visual attention in object detection , 2003, Proceedings 2003 International Conference on Image Processing (Cat. No.03CH37429).

[47]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Modeling global scene factors in attention. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[48]  Derrick J. Parkhurst,et al.  Scene content selected by active vision. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[49]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area and Spatial Attention , 2003, Science.

[50]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The time course of visual information accrual guiding eye movement decisions. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  L. Itti Author address: , 1999 .

[53]  D. Simons,et al.  Do New Objects Capture Attention? , 2005, Psychological science.

[54]  R. Abrams,et al.  The onset of receding motion captures attention: Comment on Franconeri and Simons (2003) , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[55]  Asha Iyer,et al.  Components of bottom-up gaze allocation in natural images , 2005, Vision Research.

[56]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time , 2005, Vision Research.

[57]  H. Basford,et al.  Optimal eye movement strategies in visual search , 2005 .

[58]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Neurobiological models of normal and abnormal visual development , 2005 .

[59]  T. B. Ismail,et al.  Best Paper Award , 2016, Journal of Failure Analysis and Prevention.