Effects of muscle conditioning on position sense at the human forearm during loading or fatigue of elbow flexors and the role of the sense of effort

In a forearm position‐matching task in the horizontal plane, when one (reference) arm is conditioned by contraction and length changes, subjects make systematic errors in the placement of their other, indicator arm. Here we describe experiments that demonstrate the importance not just of conditioning the reference arm, but of the indicator arm as well. Total errors from muscle conditioning represented up to a quarter of the angular range available to subjects. The sizes of the observed effects have led us to repeat other, previously reported experiments. In a matching task in the vertical plane, when muscles of both arms were conditioned identically, if the subject supported their arms themselves, or when the arms were loaded by the addition of weights, the loading did not introduce new position errors. To test the effect of exercise, subjects' elbow flexors were exercised eccentrically or concentrically by asking them to lower or raise a set of weights using forearm muscles. The exercise produced 25–30% decreases in maximum voluntary contraction strength of elbow flexors and this led to significant position‐matching errors. The directions and magnitudes of the errors were similar after the two forms of exercise and indicated that subjects perceived their exercised muscles to be longer than they actually were. To conclude, the new data from loading the arm are not consistent with the idea that the sense of effort accompanying support of a load, provides positional information in any simple way. Our current working hypothesis is that when muscles are active, position‐sense involves operation of a forward internal model. Loading the arm produces predictable changes in motor output and afferent feedback whereas changes after exercise are unpredictable. This difference leads to exercise‐dependent errors.

[1]  U. Proske,et al.  Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: A review , 1993, Progress in Neurobiology.

[2]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The role of joint receptors in human kinaesthesia when intramuscular receptors cannot contribute. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  K. R. Mills,et al.  Ultrastructural changes after concentric and eccentric contractions of human muscle , 1983, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[4]  A. Vallbo,et al.  Muscle spindle response at the onset of isometric voluntary contractions in man. Time difference between fusimotor and skeletomotor effects , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Corollary motor discharges and kinaesthesia , 1975, Brain Research.

[7]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[8]  R. Melzack,et al.  Experimental phantom limbs. , 1973, Experimental neurology.

[9]  A. Vallbo,et al.  Human muscle spindle discharge during isometric voluntary contractions. Amplitude relations between spindle frequency and torque. , 1974, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[10]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Ago-antagonist muscle spindle inputs contribute together to joint movement coding in man , 1998, Brain Research.

[11]  P. Matthews Where does Sherrington's "muscular sense" originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges? , 1982, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  J. L. Taylor,et al.  Maximal force, voluntary activation and muscle soreness after eccentric damage to human elbow flexor muscles , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  D. Newham,et al.  Pain and fatigue after concentric and eccentric muscle contractions. , 1983, Clinical science.

[15]  J. Lackner,et al.  Gravitoinertial force level affects the appreciation of limb position during muscle vibration , 1992, Brain Research.

[16]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[17]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  T. Allen,et al.  Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  K. Cullen Sensory signals during active versus passive movement , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  U. Proske,et al.  Aftereffects in the responses of cat muscle spindles and errors of limb position sense in man. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Uwe Proske,et al.  Kinesthesia: The role of muscle receptors , 2006, Muscle & nerve.

[22]  U. Proske,et al.  Effect of muscle fatigue on the sense of limb position and movement , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  U. Proske,et al.  Matching different levels of isometric torque in elbow flexor muscles after eccentric exercise , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  J Paillard,et al.  A proprioceptive contribution to the spatial encoding of position cues for ballistic movements. , 1974, Brain research.

[25]  C. Gielen,et al.  Separate control of arm position and velocity demonstrated by vibration of muscle tendon in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  U. Proske,et al.  Human forearm position sense after fatigue of elbow flexor muscles , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Lack of conscious recognition of one's own actions in a haptically deafferented patient , 2002, Neuroreport.

[29]  C. Capaday,et al.  The effects of muscle vibration on the attainment of intended final position during voluntary human arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  J A Winter,et al.  Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Muscle sense and effort: motor commands and judgments about muscular contractions. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[32]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Effect of eccentric exercise on position sense at the human forearm in different postures. , 2006, Journal of applied physiology.