The Psychological Lives of the Poor.

All individuals rely on a fundamental set of mental capacities and functions, or bandwidth, in their economic and non-economic lives. Yet, many factors associated with poverty, such as malnutrition, alcohol consumption, or sleep deprivation, may tax this capacity. Previous research has demonstrated that such taxes often significantly alter judgments, preferences, and decision-making. A more suggestive but growing body of evidence points toward potential effects on productivity and utility. Considering the lives of the poor through the lens of bandwidth may improve our understanding of potential causes and consequences of poverty.

[1]  D. Hruschka,et al.  Material wealth in 3D: Mapping multiple paths to prosperity in low- and middle- income countries , 2017, PloS one.

[2]  B. Shiv,et al.  Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making , 1999 .

[3]  S. Pailler,et al.  The effects of climate variability on psychological well-being in India , 2018 .

[4]  Uttara Balakrishnan,et al.  The Effect of a Workfare Programme on Psychological Wellbeing in India , 2018, The Journal of Development Studies.

[5]  B. Zuckerman,et al.  Child Poverty: New Opportunities for Pediatricians. , 2018, The Journal of pediatrics.

[6]  C. Steele,et al.  Alcohol myopia. Its prized and dangerous effects. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[7]  A. Ward,et al.  Don't mind if I do: disinhibited eating under cognitive load. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  E. Campbell,et al.  Subsistence difficulties are associated with more barriers to quitting and worse abstinence outcomes among homeless smokers: evidence from two studies in Boston, Massachusetts , 2018, BMC Public Health.

[9]  I. Fisher Prohibition at its worst , 1927 .

[10]  Jason L. Hicks,et al.  Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory , 1998 .

[11]  Cary Deck,et al.  The Effect of Cognitive Load on Economic Decision Making , 2015 .

[12]  Jonathan Bauchet,et al.  Microfinance bundling and consumer protection: experimental evidence from Colombia , 2017 .

[13]  E. V. Ginneken Constrained Agency: The Role of Self-Control in the Process of Desistance , 2017 .

[14]  Rebekah Russell–Bennett,et al.  Money or mind? What matters most in influencing low-income earners to be energy efficient? , 2018 .

[15]  L. Savadori,et al.  Selective insensitivity for losses but not gains in decision making under risk among the poor , 2018, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics.

[16]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[17]  Sendhil Mullainathan,et al.  Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function , 2013, Science.

[18]  P. Whitney,et al.  Impulsive decision making and working memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  S. Mullainathan,et al.  Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So Much , 2013 .

[20]  D. Fudenberg,et al.  A Dual Self Model of Impulse Control , 2004, The American economic review.

[21]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. , 2002 .

[22]  R. Marsh,et al.  Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Josef Brozek,et al.  The Biology of Human Starvation. , 1950 .

[24]  M. Pillutla,et al.  Workplace trust as a mechanism of employee (dis)advantage: The case of employee socioeconomic status , 2017 .

[25]  Jorge Cuartas,et al.  Ecological Predictors of Perceiving Scarcity in Childhood , 2019 .

[26]  Heather Schofield The Economic Costs of Low Caloric Intake: Evidence from India , 2014 .

[27]  Stephen M. Johnson,et al.  The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits , 2000 .

[28]  Carrie R. Leana,et al.  The Price of Financial Precarity: Organizational Costs of Employees' Financial Concerns , 2018, Organ. Sci..