Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection

Recent research has indicated a negative relation between the propensity for analytic reasoning and religious beliefs and practices. Here, we propose conflict detection as a mechanism underlying this relation, on the basis of the hypothesis that more-analytic people are less religious, in part, because they are more sensitive to conflicts between immaterial religious beliefs and beliefs about the material world. To examine cognitive conflict sensitivity, we presented problems containing stereotypes that conflicted with base-rate probabilities in a task with no religious content. In three studies, we found evidence that religiosity is negatively related to conflict detection during reasoning. Independent measures of analytic cognitive style also positively predicted conflict detection. The present findings provide evidence for a mechanism potentially contributing to the negative association between analytic thinking and religiosity, and more generally, they illustrate the insights to be gained from integrating individual-difference factors and contextual factors to investigate analytic reasoning.

[1]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability , 2007 .

[2]  Joseph A. Bulbulia,et al.  Toward an evolutionary social neuroscience of religion , 2011 .

[3]  Wim De Neys,et al.  Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking , 2008, Cognition.

[4]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief , 2012, Cognition.

[5]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Existential neuroscience: a proximate explanation of religion as flexible meaning and palliative , 2011 .

[6]  Gordon Pennycook,et al.  Sleep Paralysis Postepisode Distress , 2013 .

[7]  Valerie A. Thompson,et al.  Intuition, reason, and metacognition , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Reflecting on God , 2010, Psychological science.

[10]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Jonathan Evans How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many? , 2009 .

[12]  David G. Rand,et al.  Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  M. Lindeman,et al.  The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. , 2013, British journal of psychology.

[14]  Pascal Boyer,et al.  The Naturalness of Religious Ideas: A Cognitive Theory of Religion , 1995 .

[15]  W. Neys Bias and Conflict A Case for Logical Intuitions , 2012 .

[16]  Ulric Neisser,et al.  The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. , 1998 .

[17]  Scott Atran,et al.  Religion's evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  V. Goel,et al.  Smarter Than We Think , 2008, Psychological science.

[19]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Individual differences in rational thought. , 1998 .

[20]  Joseph A. Bulbulia,et al.  Toward an evolutionary social neuroscience of religion , 2011 .

[21]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[23]  V. Thompson Dual-process theories: A metacognitive perspective , 2009 .

[24]  Rakefet Ackerman,et al.  The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues for initiating analytic thinking , 2013, Cognition.

[25]  Nathan Novemsky,et al.  How are base-rates used, when they are used: a comparison of additive and Bayesian models of base-rate use , 1999 .

[26]  Robert M. Hauser,et al.  Trends in Black-White Test Score Differentials: II. The WORDSUM Vocabulary Test , 1998 .

[27]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[28]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Rationality and reasoning , 1996 .

[29]  Kyle Nash,et al.  Neural Markers of Religious Conviction , 2009, Psychological science.

[30]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[31]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  DISPOSITIONAL DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE MOTIVATION : THE LIFE AND TIMES OF INDIVIDUALS VARYING IN NEED FOR COGNITION , 1996 .

[32]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  The Robot's Rebellion: Finding Meaning in the Age of Darwin , 2004 .

[33]  K. Jellinger,et al.  In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. , 2009 .

[34]  R. Hauser,et al.  Trends in Black-White Test-Score Differentials , 1996 .

[35]  The need to believe in conflicting propositions , 2011 .

[36]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[37]  Will M. Gervais,et al.  Analytic Thinking Promotes Religious Disbelief , 2012, Science.

[38]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Are we good at detecting conflict during reasoning? , 2012, Cognition.

[39]  Belief bias during reasoning among religious believers and skeptics , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  A. Tversky,et al.  On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .

[43]  K. Stanovich,et al.  The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks , 2011, Memory & cognition.