Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials
暂无分享,去创建一个
Michael Falkenstein | Joachim Hohnsbein | Nele Wild-Wall | J. Hohnsbein | M. Falkenstein | N. Wild-Wall
[1] Antigona Martínez,et al. Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[2] Lynn Hasher,et al. Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .
[3] Alexa B. Roggeveen,et al. Lateralized readiness potentials reveal motor slowing in the aging brain. , 2007, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[4] J. Lupiáñez,et al. Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials , 2006, Brain Research.
[5] C. Rosano,et al. Functional neuroimaging indicators of successful executive control in the oldest old , 2005, NeuroImage.
[6] Jeff Miller,et al. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[7] A. Osman,et al. Mechanisms of speed–accuracy tradeoff: evidence from covert motor processes , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[8] Borís Burle,et al. Deficit in motor cortical activity for simultaneous bimanual responses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] B. Kopp,et al. N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[10] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of choice complexity on different subcomponents of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[11] L. S. Miller,et al. Age-related decrements in performance on a brief continuous performance test. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[12] Sarah E. MacPherson,et al. Age, executive function, and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. , 2002, Psychology and aging.
[13] Jens C. Pruessner,et al. Regional Frontal Cortical Volumes Decrease Differentially in Aging: An MRI Study to Compare Volumetric Approaches and Voxel-Based Morphometry , 2002, NeuroImage.
[14] Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] Diego Fernandez-Duque,et al. Attentional networks in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. , 2006, Neuropsychology.
[16] Robert W Proctor,et al. The enhanced Simon effect for older adults is reduced when the irrelevant location information is conveyed by an accessory stimulus. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[17] Jeff Miller,et al. Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[18] A Kok,et al. Age-related differences in the timing of stimulus and response processes during visual selective attention: performance and psychophysiological analyses. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[19] A. Kok. Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected in components of the event-related potential (ERP) , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[20] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.
[21] R. Knight,et al. Prefrontal deficits in attention and inhibitory control with aging. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[22] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[23] T. Braver,et al. A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[24] R. Knight,et al. Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] M. Coles. Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[26] Cheryl L. Dickter,et al. Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: beyond errors and response conflict. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[27] Huifang Wang,et al. Activation of response codes by relevant and irrelevant stimulus information , 1995 .
[28] G. Mulder,et al. Focussing on aging: an electrophysiological exploration of spatial and attentional processing during reading , 1996, Biological Psychology.
[29] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Mental resources, processing speed, and inhibitory control: a developmental perspective , 1997, Biological Psychology.
[30] A Kok,et al. Varieties of inhibition: manifestations in cognition, event-related potentials and aging. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[31] C M Moore,et al. Bisecting RT with lateralized readiness potentials: precue effects of LRP onset. , 1995, Acta psychologica.
[32] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[33] S J Luck,et al. Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[34] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[35] T. Salthouse. Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[36] Rolf Verleger,et al. Spatial S-R Compatibility with Centrally Presented Stimuli: An Event-Related Asymmetry Study on Dimensional Overlap , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[38] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. , 2001 .
[39] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[40] R. Verleger,et al. Aging and the Simon task. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[41] M. Falkenstein,et al. Effects of aging on slowing of motor-response generation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[42] E Donchin,et al. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[43] Michael Falkenstein,et al. ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm , 2008, Brain Research.
[44] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Event-related potentials reveal dissociable mechanisms for orienting and focusing visuospatial attention. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[45] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Limits on the application of additive factors logic: Violations of stage robustness suggest a dual-process architecture to explain flanker effects on target processing , 1995 .
[46] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[47] A Kok,et al. Perceptual factors affecting age-related differences in focused attention: performance and psychophysiological analyses. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[48] S Pollmann,et al. ERP and fMRI correlates of endogenous and exogenous focusing of visual‐spatial attention , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.
[49] R. West,et al. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.
[50] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Central and parietal event-related lateralizations in a flanker task. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[51] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] T Hasbroucq,et al. Response selection and motor areas: a behavioural and electrophysiological study , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.