Self-to-Prototype Matching as a Strategy for Making Academic Choices. Why High School Students Do Not like Math and Science.

The percentage ofschool students specializing in math and science is particularly low. The current research suggests that this is due to prototypes about math and science being highly dissimilar from self-prototypes students have or want to have of themselves. Going beyond previous studies on self-to-prototype matching, we assumed that students compare their selfviews to both a prototypical student liking a certain subject (favourite-subject-prototype) and a prototypical student disapproving ofit ( least liked subject-prototype). Results show that for humanities (German and English language), favourite-subject-prototypes were judged more positively than least-liked-subject-prototypes, whereas for science (math and physics), leastliked-subject-prototypes were perceived as more positive than favourite-subject-prototypes. As expected, only if a student’s self-prototype was quite clear (high self-clarity) was it used as a standard against which school-subject prototypes were compared with respect to their degree ofoverlap. Our results showed that the better the match between selfand f subject-prototype, the stronger were the subject preferences. # 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

[1]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  "Who am I? Why am I here?": self-esteem, self-clarity, and prototype matching. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Kai U. Schnabel,et al.  Studienfachwünsche und Berufsorientierungen in der gymnasialen Oberstufe , 2000 .

[3]  J. D. Campbell,et al.  Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Nancy Cantor,et al.  A prototype analysis of psychological situations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  Prototype Distancing: A Strategy for Choosing among Threatening Situations , 1991 .

[6]  D. R. Lehman,et al.  Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. , 1996 .

[7]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social Psychology , 2019, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[8]  Ingrid Schoon,et al.  Teenage job aspirations and career attainment in adulthood: A 17-year follow-up study of teenagers who aspired to become scientists, health professionals, or engineers , 2001 .

[9]  John F. Ohles,et al.  Interest and Learning. , 1969 .

[10]  J. Kihlstrom,et al.  Mental Representations of the Self , 1984 .

[11]  D. Hamilton Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior , 1981 .

[12]  J. Kihlstrom,et al.  Prototype matching: a strategy for social decision making. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  R. Dawis,et al.  Measurement of person-environment fit and prediction of satisfaction in the theory of work adjustment☆ , 1987 .

[14]  J. L. Holland,et al.  Making vocational choices : a theory of vocational personalities and work environments , 1984 .

[15]  James D. Lee Which Kids Can ‘Become’ Scientists? Effects of Gender, Self-Concepts and Perceptions of Scientists , 1998 .

[16]  W. Mischel,et al.  Prototypes in Person Perception1 , 1979 .

[17]  D. Ruble,et al.  A phase model of transitions: cognitive and motivational consequences , 1994 .

[18]  Bettina Hannover,et al.  Monoedukativer Anfangsunterricht in Physik in der Gesamtschule , 2002 .

[19]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender Stereotypes and Attitudes Toward Women and Men , 1989 .

[20]  Irene Hanson Frieze,et al.  Job preferences in the anticipatory socialization phase: A comparison of two matching models. , 1993 .

[21]  Bettina Hannover,et al.  Challenge the science-stereotype. Der Einfluss von Technik-Freizeitkursen auf das Naturwissenschaften-Stereotyp von Schülerinnen und Schülern , 2002 .