Attentional modulation of perceptual grouping in human visual cortex: ERP studies

We sought to identify the neural substrates underlying perceptual grouping, and examined whether the grouping‐related neural activities are modulated by task relevance and attention by recording high‐density event‐related potentials (ERPs). Participants were presented with stimulus arrays, in which local elements were either evenly distributed or grouped into rows or columns by proximity or similarity, and had to discriminate orientations of the perceptual groups, or to identify the colors of dots around stimulus displays or of the fixation cross. We found that proximity grouping was indexed by a positive activity over the medial occipital cortex with a peak latency of about 100 ms after stimulus onset (Pd100), whereas grouping by similarity of shape was reflected in a negative activity with longer latency over the occipito‐temporal areas. Dipole modeling based on a realistic boundary element head model localized the Pd100 to the right calcarine cortex. Moreover, we showed that the grouping‐related activities were weakened when stimulus arrays were of low task relevance and fell outside an attended area of field. The results suggest that human calcarine cortex is engaged in early grouping operations defined by proximity, reinforcing the previous fMRI findings. Moreover, our ERP results indicate that the neural bases underlying perceptual grouping in human visual cortex can be modulated by task relevance and attention as early as 100 ms after sensory stimulation. Hum Brain Mapp, 2005. © 2005 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  D L Woods,et al.  Neural substrates for visual perceptual grouping in humans. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[2]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Neural representation of objects in space: a dual coding account. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[3]  M. Wertheimer Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II , 1923 .

[4]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Properties, Parts, and Objects , 1986 .

[5]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[6]  Lihua Mao,et al.  Attentional modulation of perceptual grouping in human visual cortex: Functional MRI studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[7]  I. Rock,et al.  Perceptual organization and attention , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  Yoichi Sugita,et al.  Grouping of image fragments in primary visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[9]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Parallel and competitive processes in hierarchical analysis: perceptual grouping and encoding of closure. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Perceptual Organization of Local Elements into Global Shapes in the Human Visual Cortex , 2003, Current Biology.

[11]  D. Sagi,et al.  Visual attention and perceptual grouping , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Scene segmentation and attention in primate cortical areas V1 and V2. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  G. Mangun,et al.  Covariations in ERP and PET measures of spatial selective attention in human extrastriate visual cortex , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[14]  D. Sagi,et al.  Perceptual grouping by similarity and proximity: Experimental results can be predicted by intensity autocorrelations , 1995, Vision Research.

[15]  M. Behrmann,et al.  6 Attention and unit formation: A biased competition account of object-based attention , 2001 .

[16]  Yulong Ding,et al.  Neural mechanisms of perceptual grouping in humans as revealed by high density event related potentials , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  M Fahle,et al.  Correlates of figure-ground segregation in fMRI , 2000, Vision Research.

[18]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Preattentive Filling-in of Visual Surfaces in Parietal Extinction , 1997, Science.

[19]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Delayed Striate Cortical Activation during Spatial Attention , 2002, Neuron.

[21]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[22]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Uniform connectedness and classical gestalt principles of perceptual grouping , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  Jon Driver,et al.  Grouping reduces visual extinction: Neuropsychological evidence for weight-linkage in visual selection , 1994 .

[24]  H. Egeth,et al.  Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Richard B Buxton,et al.  Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.

[26]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[27]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[28]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[29]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Spatial Selective Attention Affects Early Extrastriate But Not Striate Components of the Visual Evoked Potential , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Y. Tsal,et al.  On the status of location in visual attention , 2001 .

[31]  Tutis Vilis,et al.  The lateral occipital complex subserves the perceptual persistence of motion-defined groupings. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Interactions between perceptual organization based on Gestalt laws and those based on hierarchical processing , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.