High Resolution Human Eye Tracking During Continuous Visual Search

While several studies have shown human subjects’ impressive ability to detect faces in individual images in paced settings (Crouzet et al., 2010), we here report the details of an eye movement dataset in which subjects rapidly and continuously targeted single faces embedded in different scenes at rates approaching six face targets each second (including blinks and eye movement times). In this paper, we describe details of a large publicly available eye movement dataset of this new psychophysical paradigm (Martin et al., 2018). The paradigm produced high-resolution eye-tracking data from an experiment on continuous upright and inverted 3° sized face detection in both background and no-background conditions. The new “Zapping” paradigm allowed large amounts of trials to be completed in a short amount of time. For example, our three studies encompassed a total of 288,000 trials done in 72 separate experiments, and yet only took approximately 40 hours of recording for the three experimental cohorts. Each subject did 4000 trials split into eight blocks of 500 consecutive trials in one of the four different experimental conditions: {upright, inverted} × {scene, no scene}. For each condition, there are several covariates of interest, including: temporal eye positions sampled at 1250 hz, saccades, saccade reaction times, microsaccades, pupil dynamics, target luminances, and global contrasts.

[1]  Masaaki Kawahashi,et al.  Renovation of Journal of Visualization , 2010, J. Vis..

[2]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Coregistration of eye movements and EEG in natural reading: analyses and review. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  K. Nakayama,et al.  The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area , 1998, Cognition.

[4]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[5]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Neural Basis of the Behavioral Face-Inversion Effect , 2005, Current Biology.

[6]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  U. Neisser VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.

[8]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[9]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  What neural pathways mediate express saccades? , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception , 1999, Neuron.

[11]  Denis Fize,et al.  Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.

[12]  Sébastien M. Crouzet,et al.  Fast saccades toward faces: face detection in just 100 ms. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[13]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  Charles E. Davis,et al.  Zapping 500 faces in less than 100 seconds: Evidence for extremely fast and sustained continuous visual search , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[15]  Jatinder Singh,et al.  FigShare , 2011, Journal of pharmacology & pharmacotherapeutics.

[16]  Nao Ninomiya,et al.  The 10th anniversary of journal of visualization , 2007, J. Vis..

[17]  C. Helfrich-Förster,et al.  Drosophila timeless2 Is Required for Chromosome Stability and Circadian Photoreception , 2010, Current Biology.

[18]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Timing of saccadic eye movements during visual search for multiple targets. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[19]  Radoslaw Mantiuk Accuracy of High-End and Self-build Eye-Tracking Systems , 2016, ACS.