The coincidence between late non-phase-locked gamma synchronization response and saccadic eye movements.

The event-related response in the gamma (30-45 Hz) frequency band was studied in healthy subjects (n=45) viewing sequentially presented pictures from the International Affective Picture System. The distinct non-phase-locked gamma response was obtained in characteristic time window (200-400 ms) with clear-cut centro-parietal location. The strong coincidence between induced gamma oscillations and saccadic eye movements was revealed. We suggest that saccade-related gamma increase is another manifestation of the phenomenon known as presaccadic spike potential, which is commonly registered over parietal scalp leads at 10-20 ms prior to saccade onset. It is hypothesized that late non-phase-locked gamma synchronization mainly reflects activity of a system responsible for attentional tuning and motor planning/execution of saccadic eye movements.

[1]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[2]  Perisaccadic Occipital EEG Changes Quantified with Wavelet Analysis , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  R. Thatcher Functional neuroimaging : technical foundations , 1994 .

[4]  E. Gordon,et al.  Synchronous Gamma activity: a review and contribution to an integrative neuroscience model of schizophrenia , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[5]  E. Basar,et al.  Gamma, alpha, delta, and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[6]  W. Skrandies,et al.  Electroencephalographic cortical oscillations and saccadic eye movements in humans , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  T Gasser,et al.  The analysis of the EEG , 1996, Statistical methods in medical research.

[8]  C. Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Gamma response of the brain: a multifunctional oscillation that represents bottom-up with top-down processing. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[9]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[10]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Effects of emotional arousal in the cerebral hemispheres: a study of oscillatory brain activity and event-related potentials , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[11]  John E. Richards,et al.  Localizing the development of covert attention in infants with scalp event-related potentials. , 2000 .

[12]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Barry,et al.  Removal of ocular artifact from the EEG: a review , 2000, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  M Schürmann,et al.  Selectively distributed gamma band system of the brain. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  P. Lang International Affective Picture System (IAPS) : Technical Manual and Affective Ratings , 1995 .

[17]  G. Thickbroom,et al.  Presaccadic ‘spike’ potential: Investigation of topography and source , 1985, Brain Research.

[18]  C. Stefanis,et al.  An early transient 40 Hz activity discriminates a following pro-saccade from a no-move and anti-saccade choice , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  K. Sauvé Gamma-Band Synchronous Oscillations: Recent Evidence Regarding Their Functional Significance , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  Carey D. Balaban,et al.  Directional tuning of the human presaccadic spike potential , 1991, Brain Research.

[21]  G. Goldberg,et al.  Topography of scalp potentials preceding self‐initiated saccades , 1990, Neurology.

[22]  O. Bertrand,et al.  Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Cortical control of ocular saccades in humans: a model for motricity. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[24]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[25]  C. Pantev Evoked and induced gamma-band activity of the human cortex , 2005, Brain Topography.

[26]  F. Wolf-Oberhollenzer,et al.  Cortical oscillations and the origin of express saccades , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in humans , 1993, Nature.

[29]  F. Riemslag,et al.  On the origin of the presaccadic spike potential. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The role of the parietal cortex in the neural processing of saccadic eye movements. , 2003, Advances in neurology.

[31]  N. I. Nezlina,et al.  The Orientating Reflex: The “Targeting Reaction” and “Searchlight of Attention” , 2002, Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology.