Orientation tuning in human colour vision at detection threshold

We measure the orientation tuning of red-green colour and luminance vision at low (0.375 c/deg) and mid (1.5 c/deg) spatial frequencies using the low-contrast psychophysical method of subthreshold summation. Orientation bandwidths of the underlying neural detectors are found using a model involving Minkowski summation of the rectified outputs of a bank of oriented filters. At 1.5 c/deg, we find orientation-tuned detectors with similar bandwidths for chromatic and achromatic contrast. At 0.375 c/deg, orientation tuning is preserved with no change in bandwidth for achromatic stimuli, however, for chromatic stimuli orientation tuning becomes extremely broad, compatible with detection by non-oriented colour detectors. A non-oriented colour detector, previously reported in single cells in primate V1 but not psychophysically in humans, can transmit crucial information about the color of larger areas or surfaces whereas orientation-tuned detectors are required to detect the colour or luminance edges that delineate an object's shape.

[1]  Arthur Bradley,et al.  Orientation and spatial frequency selectivity of adaptation to color and luminance gratings , 1988, Vision Research.

[2]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Event-related potential evidence for a dual-locus model of global/local processing , 2010 .

[3]  C. Tyler,et al.  Signal detection theory in the 2AFC paradigm: attention, channel uncertainty and probability summation , 2000, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Robson,et al.  Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field , 1981, Vision Research.

[5]  Yaniv Morgenstern,et al.  Local Visual Energy Mechanisms Revealed by Detection of Global Patterns , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Ringach Spatial structure and symmetry of simple-cell receptive fields in macaque primary visual cortex. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  A. Watson,et al.  A standard model for foveal detection of spatial contrast. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[8]  S. Engel,et al.  Color opponent neurons in V1: a review and model reconciling results from imaging and single-unit recording. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[9]  K. Mullen,et al.  Cross-orientation masking in human color vision: application of a two-stage model to assess dichoptic and monocular sources of suppression. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[10]  D. Hubel,et al.  Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. , 1966, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Separate channels for processing form, texture, and color: evidence from FMRI adaptation and visual object agnosia. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Computational models of visual processing , 1991 .

[13]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Binocular contrast summation—II. Quadratic summation , 1984, Vision Research.

[14]  H. Wilson,et al.  Orientation bandwidths of spatial mechanisms measured by masking. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  K. Mullen,et al.  Orientation selectivity in luminance and color vision assessed using 2-d band-pass filtered spatial noise , 2005, Vision Research.

[16]  K. Mullen,et al.  Estimation of the L-, M-, and S-cone weights of the postreceptoral detection mechanisms , 1996 .

[17]  Kathy T Mullen,et al.  Blobs versus bars: psychophysical evidence supports two types of orientation response in human color vision. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[18]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[19]  R. L. Pandey Vimal,et al.  Orientation tuning of the spatial-frequency-tuned mechanisms of the red-green channel. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[20]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[21]  F. Campbell,et al.  Orientational selectivity of the human visual system , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  T. Meese,et al.  Probability summation for multiple patches of luminance modulation , 2000, Vision Research.

[23]  R. Shapley,et al.  Color in the Cortex: single- and double-opponent cells , 2011, Vision Research.

[24]  R. L. Vimal,et al.  Spatial-frequency tuning of sustained nonoriented units of the red-green channel. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[25]  Quick Rf A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .

[26]  R. Shapley,et al.  The Orientation Selectivity of Color-Responsive Neurons in Macaque V1 , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  E. Essock The Oblique Effect of Stimulus Identification Considered with Respect to Two Classes of Oblique Effects , 1980, Perception.

[28]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  D. Hubel,et al.  Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  K. Mullen,et al.  Cross-orientation masking in human color vision. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[31]  D. Alais,et al.  Orientation-Specificity of Adaptation: Isotropic Adaptation Is Purely Monocular , 2012, PloS one.

[32]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Orientation Specificity and Spatial Selectivity in Human Vision , 1973, Perception.

[33]  Nicholas V. Swindale,et al.  Orientation tuning curves: empirical description and estimation of parameters , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  K. V. Mardia,et al.  Algorithm AS 86: The Von Mises Distribution Function , 1975 .

[35]  Robert J Summers,et al.  Neuronal convergence in early contrast vision: binocular summation is followed by response nonlinearity and area summation. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[36]  J. Robson,et al.  Spatial-frequency channels in human vision. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[37]  H. Wilson,et al.  Spatial frequency tuning of orientation selective units estimated by oblique masking , 1983, Vision Research.

[38]  Ipek Oruc,et al.  Improved face discrimination after face adaptation , 2009 .

[39]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The orientation specificity of two visual after‐effects , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[40]  K. Mullen,et al.  The spatial tuning of chromatic mechanisms identified by simultaneous masking , 1994, Vision Research.

[41]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Orientation selectivity in cats and humans assessed by masking , 1985, Vision Research.

[42]  J. Bergen,et al.  A four mechanism model for threshold spatial vision , 1979, Vision Research.

[43]  T. Meese,et al.  Orientation masking and cross-orientation suppression (XOS): implications for estimates of filter bandwidth. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[44]  J. M. Foley,et al.  Human luminance pattern-vision mechanisms: masking experiments require a new model. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[45]  Brian J. Murphy,et al.  Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions , 1980, Vision Research.

[46]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[47]  W. McIlhagga,et al.  Detection mechanisms in L-, M-, and S-cone contrast space. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[48]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Orientation and spatial-frequency discrimination for luminance and chromatic gratings. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[49]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Spatial mapping of monkey VI cells with pure color and luminance stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[50]  T. Meese Spatially extensive summation of contrast energy is revealed by contrast detection of micro-pattern textures. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[51]  J P Thomas,et al.  Bandwidths of orientation channels in human vision. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[52]  Hong Zhou,et al.  The coding of uniform colour figures in monkey visual cortex , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[53]  Sophie M. Wuerger,et al.  Input of long- and middle-wavelength-sensitive cones to orientation discrimination , 1999 .

[54]  H R Wilson,et al.  Further evidence for four mechanisms mediating vision at threshold: sensitivities to complex gratings and aperiodic stimuli. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[55]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Spatial and Temporal Properties of Cone Signals in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  On the relation between summation and facilitation , 1987, Vision Research.

[57]  D. Burr,et al.  Spatial and temporal selectivity of the human motion detection system , 1985, Vision Research.

[58]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Spatial arrangement of line, edge and grating detectors revealed by subthreshold summation. , 1973, Vision research.

[59]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Perceived contrast of gratings and plaids: Non-linear summation across oriented filters , 1994, Vision Research.

[60]  R. Quick A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection , 2004, Kybernetik.

[61]  D. Alais,et al.  Orientation bandwidths are invariant across spatiotemporal frequency after isotropic components are removed. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[62]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Orientational selectivity of grating and line detectors in human vision. , 1973, Vision research.

[63]  Gregory D Horwitz,et al.  V1 mechanisms underlying chromatic contrast detection. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.