A multimodal study to measure the cognitive demands of hazard recognition in construction workplaces

Abstract Hazard recognition has been extensively explored in previous studies. However, deficits have arisen due to the neglect of task-specific effects, information distortion by image-based experimental tasks, and the exclusive use of eye trackers. This study aimed to explore how cognitive patterns vary in simulated construction worksites with different types of hazards using multimodal monitoring. A hazard recognition task was conducted in a hazardous civil laboratory using both an eye tracker and a near-infrared spectrum system to capture pupil responses and cerebral oxyhemoglobin signals. Cognitive responses were analyzed according to hazard type and scene complexity. The results showed that falling hazards induced the most cerebral and pupillary activation. Scene complexity triggers an increase in pupil diameter and impacts cerebral activities by interaction with hazard type. This study also reveals the complementary functions of pupillary responses and neural processes in hazardous simulated worksites and a ceiling effect of cognitive resources. We conclude that construction workplaces with different types of hazards can induce different cognitive demands and should thus be treated individually. This information is potentially useful for practical applications.

[1]  Fadilla Zennifa,et al.  Hybrid System for Engagement Recognition During Cognitive Tasks Using a CFS + KNN Algorithm , 2018, Sensors.

[2]  Jens Rasmussen,et al.  Risk management in a dynamic society: a modelling problem , 1997 .

[3]  Susanne Bahn,et al.  Workplace hazard identification and management: The case of an underground mining operation , 2013 .

[4]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Differential prefrontal and frontotemporal oxygenation patterns during phonemic and semantic verbal fluency , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Jean-Marc Robert,et al.  Effects of visual clutter on pilot workload, flight performance and gaze pattern , 2014 .

[6]  Chin-Teng Lin,et al.  Using eye-tracker to compare search patterns between experienced and novice workers for site hazard identification , 2016 .

[7]  Judit Kormos,et al.  The role of working memory in processing L2 input: Insights from eye-tracking , 2017, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[8]  Antoine J.-P. Tixier,et al.  Psychological Antecedents of Risk-Taking Behavior in Construction , 2014 .

[9]  Simon Smith,et al.  Safety hazard identification on construction projects , 2006 .

[10]  Alessandro Torricelli,et al.  Bilateral prefrontal cortex oxygenation responses to a verbal fluency task: a multichannel time-resolved near-infrared topography study. , 2005, Journal of biomedical optics.

[11]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Michail N. Giannakos,et al.  Multimodal data as a means to understand the learning experience , 2019, Int. J. Inf. Manag..

[13]  Baizhan Li,et al.  Management of climatic heat stress risk in construction: a review of practices, methodologies, and future research. , 2014, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[14]  E. Granholm,et al.  Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Ranjana K Mehta,et al.  Methodological Approaches and Recommendations for Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Applications in HF/E Research , 2020, Hum. Factors.

[16]  David A. Boas,et al.  A Quantitative Comparison of Simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS Recordings during Functional Brain Activation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[17]  Pin-Chao Liao,et al.  Influence of visual clutter on the effect of navigated safety inspection: a case study on elevator installation , 2019, International journal of occupational safety and ergonomics : JOSE.

[18]  Stephen R Mitroff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Attentional Guidance by Working Memory Overrides Salience Cues in Visual Search , 2013 .

[19]  Kara K. Patterson,et al.  Oxyhemoglobin changes in the prefrontal cortex in response to cognitive tasks: a systematic review , 2018, The International journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Impact of Construction Workers’ Hazard Identification Skills on Their Visual Attention , 2017 .

[21]  Bhavana Pandit,et al.  Impact of safety climate on hazard recognition and safety risk perception , 2019, Safety Science.

[22]  A. Gunji,et al.  Scanning Strategies Do Not Modulate Face Identification: Eye-Tracking and Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study , 2010, PloS one.

[23]  Xinyi Song,et al.  Revealing the "invisible Gorilla" in construction: Estimating construction safety through mental workload assessment , 2016 .

[24]  Matthew R. Hallowell,et al.  Enhancing Construction Hazard Recognition and Communication with Energy-Based Cognitive Mnemonics and Safety Meeting Maturity Model: Multiple Baseline Study , 2014 .

[25]  Nicolas Pinto,et al.  Why is Real-World Visual Object Recognition Hard? , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[26]  Michael Klesel,et al.  A test for multigroup comparison using partial least squares path modeling , 2019, Internet Res..

[27]  Pin-Chao Liao,et al.  Efficiency improvement by navigated safety inspection involving visual clutter based on the random search model , 2018, International journal of occupational safety and ergonomics : JOSE.

[28]  J. C. Gerdes,et al.  Neural, physiological, and behavioral correlates of visuomotor cognitive load , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[29]  Tao Liu,et al.  Near-infrared spectroscopy as a tool for driving research , 2016, Ergonomics.

[30]  Rafael Sacks,et al.  Hazard recognition and risk perception in construction , 2014 .

[31]  Pin-Chao Liao,et al.  Re-assessing hazard recognition ability in occupational environment with microvascular function in the brain , 2019 .

[32]  A. Roy Duff,et al.  Development of Causal Model of Construction Accident Causation , 2001 .

[33]  David Borys,et al.  The role of safe work method statements in the Australian construction industry , 2012 .

[34]  S. Tremblay,et al.  Using near infrared spectroscopy and heart rate variability to detect mental overload , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[36]  M. Bradley,et al.  The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[37]  Michael Behm,et al.  Role of Safety Training: Impact on Hazard Recognition and Safety Risk Perception , 2016 .

[38]  Ichiro Miyai,et al.  Prefrontal and premotor cortices are involved in adapting walking and running speed on the treadmill: an optical imaging study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  K. Kubota,et al.  Cortical Mapping of Gait in Humans: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopic Topography Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  A R Duff,et al.  Contributing factors in construction accidents. , 2005, Applied ergonomics.

[41]  Rafael Sacks,et al.  Construction Job Safety Analysis , 2010 .

[42]  Michael C Hout,et al.  Target templates: the precision of mental representations affects attentional guidance and decision-making in visual search , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[43]  Claudio M. Privitera,et al.  Decision-Level Fusion of EEG and Pupil Features for Single-Trial Visual Detection Analysis , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[44]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.