Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli.

We investigated the functional anatomy involved in sustaining or switching visual attention between different perceptual levels, using functional imaging measures of neural activity. Two experiments were carried out using hierarchically organized letters (i.e. large letters made out of small letters). In a divided-attention task, subjects were required to switch attention between local and global levels. The number of successive stimuli for which subjects had to sustain attention to either the global or local level co-varied significantly with temporal-parietal activations bilaterally. Other activations were also observed in the right orbitofrontal cortex, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the right middle temporal gyrus. The number of switches between levels co-varied significantly with activations in the left supplementary motor area and the left medial parietal cortex. In the directed-attention task, subjects were required to attend to either the global or local level of the stimuli throughout all trials; attention to the global aspect resulted in significant activation of the right lingual gyrus while attention to the local aspect significantly activated the left inferior occipital cortex. We suggest that left hemisphere activations with increasing numbers of switches between perceptual levels reflect increased demands on an executive attentional system, while sustained attention to either level activates a predominantly right hemispheric network involving temporal-parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal regions. Overall, the results provide evidence for relative hemispheric specialization for global and local processing in accordance with previous neuropsychological studies. In addition, the findings demonstrate that early visual processing mechanisms in the prestriate cortex are influenced by an attentional system in temporal-parietal areas.

[1]  G L Shulman,et al.  The Role of Spatial-Frequency Channels in the Perception of Local and Global Structure , 1986, Perception.

[2]  S. Clarke,et al.  Modular Organization of Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex: Evidence from Cytochrome Oxidase Pattern in Normal and Macular Degeneration Cases , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? , 1996, Nature.

[4]  J. Sergent About face: left-hemisphere involvement in processing physiognomies. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  M. Posner Attention in cognitive neuroscience: An overview. , 1995 .

[6]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[7]  J E Hoffman,et al.  Interaction between global and local levels of a form. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex , 1995, Nature.

[9]  E T Davis,et al.  Allocation of attention: Uncertainty effects when monitoring one or two visual gratings of noncontiguous spatial frequencies , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  A Review of Hemispheric Asymmetry: What's Right and What's Left , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  L. Robertson,et al.  Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  E. Marg A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .

[13]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  M. Mintun,et al.  Noninvasive functional brain mapping by change-distribution analysis of averaged PET images of H215O tissue activity. , 1989, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[15]  S. Rapcsak,et al.  Hierarchical Stimuli and Hemispheric Specialization: Two Case Studies , 1994, Cortex.

[16]  D L Bailey,et al.  Fully Three-dimensional Reconstruction For A Pet Camera With Retractable Septa , 1990, 1990 IEEE Nuclear Science Symposium Conference Record.

[17]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[18]  Elisabeth A. Murray,et al.  Supplementary Sensory Area The Medial Parietal Cortex in the Monkey , 1981 .

[19]  Maryanne Martin Local and global processing: The role of sparsity , 1979 .

[20]  R. Knight,et al.  Component mechanisms underlying the processing of hierarchically organized patterns: inferences from patients with unilateral cortical lesions. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  L. Robertson,et al.  Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  M. Raichle,et al.  Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography , 1991, Nature.

[23]  G. Deutsch,et al.  Cerebral blood flow evidence of right frontal activation in attention demanding tasks. , 1987, The International journal of neuroscience.

[24]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  The neurology of visual attention. , 1995 .

[25]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[26]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  L C Robertson,et al.  The processing of hierarchical stimuli: Effects of retinal locus, locational uncertainty, and stimulus identity , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Toward a principled explanation of unilateral neglect , 1994 .

[29]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Detection of Thirty-Second Cognitive Activations in Single Subjects with Positron Emission Tomography: A New Low-Dose H215O Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Three-Dimensional Imaging Technique , 1993, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[30]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. , 1988, Science.

[31]  S. Tipper,et al.  Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention , 1994 .

[32]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  A Noninvasive Positron Computed Tomography Technique Using Oxygen-15-Labeled Water for the Evaluation of Neurobehavioral Task Batteries , 1985, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[33]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  H. Heinze,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of hierarchical stimulus processing: Dissociation between onset and later stages of global and local target processing , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[38]  R. A. Kinchla,et al.  Attending to different levels of structure in a visual image , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  H. Hughes,et al.  Global Precedence, Spatial Frequency Channels, and the Statistics of Natural Images , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[41]  T Shallice,et al.  The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  T. Shallice,et al.  Frontal lesions and sustained attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  D R Badcock,et al.  Low-Frequency Filtering and the Processing of Local—Global Stimuli , 1990, Perception.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[45]  Roger Whitehead,et al.  Right Hemisphere Processing Superiority During Sustained Visual Attention , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  J. Grafman,et al.  Sustained attention deficits in pat ients with right frontal lesions , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Retinotopic Maps in Human Prestriate Visual Cortex: The Demarcation of Areas V2 and V3 , 1995, NeuroImage.

[48]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  ‘Part-whole’ processing in unilateral brain- damaged patients: Dysfunction of hierarchical organization , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[50]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Global Versus Local Processing in the Absence of Low Spatial Frequencies , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[52]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Hemispheric Differences in Auditory Perception Are Similar to Those Found in Visual Perception , 1993 .