Developmental changes in semantic knowledge organization.

Semantic knowledge is a crucial aspect of higher cognition. Theoretical accounts of semantic knowledge posit that relations between concepts provide organizational structure that converts information known about individual entities into an interconnected network in which concepts can be linked by many types of relations (e.g., taxonomic, thematic). The goal of the current research was to address several methodological shortcomings of prior studies on the development of semantic organization, by using a variant of the spatial arrangement method (SpAM) to collect graded judgments of relatedness for a set of entities that can be cross-classified into either taxonomic or thematic groups. In Experiment 1, we used the cross-classify SpAM (CC-SpAM) to obtain graded relatedness judgments and derive a representation of developmental changes in the organization of semantic knowledge. In Experiment 2, we validated the findings of Experiment 1 by using a more traditional pairwise similarity judgment paradigm. Across both experiments, we found that an early recognition of links between entities that are both taxonomically and thematically related preceded an increasing recognition of links based on a single type of relation. The utility of CC-SpAM for evaluating theoretical accounts of semantic development is discussed.

[1]  Miriam Bassok,et al.  What Makes a Man Similar to a Tie? Stimulus Compatibility with Comparison and Integration , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Optimal foraging in semantic memory. , 2012, Psychological review.

[3]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Categorical structure among shared features in networks of early-learned nouns , 2009, Cognition.

[4]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  D. Bjorklund,et al.  Associative and categorical processes in children's memory: The role of automaticity in the development of organization in free recall ☆ , 1985 .

[6]  G. Deák,et al.  The effects of task comprehension on preschoolers' and adults' categorization choices. , 1995, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

[7]  D. Faulkner,et al.  Perceptual, thematic and taxonomic relations in children’s mental representations: Responses to triads , 1993 .

[8]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age ☆ , 1979 .

[9]  Gregory L Murphy,et al.  An apple is more than just a fruit: cross-classification in children's concepts. , 2003, Child development.

[10]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Development of taxonomic organization of named and pictured categories. , 1985 .

[11]  David G. Lowe,et al.  Object recognition from local scale-invariant features , 1999, Proceedings of the Seventh IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision.

[12]  Agnès Blaye,et al.  Categorical flexibility in children: Distinguishing response flexibility from conceptual flexibility; the protracted development of taxonomic representations , 2006 .

[13]  Ken McRae,et al.  Category - Specific semantic deficits , 2008 .

[14]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[15]  Christine Storm The Semantic Structure of Animal Terms: A Developmental Study , 1980 .

[16]  G. Bower,et al.  Human Associative Memory , 1973 .

[17]  Sau-chin Chen,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 2018 .

[18]  K. McGregor,et al.  Conceptual organization at 6 and 8 years of age: evidence from the semantic priming of object decisions. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[19]  D. Entwisle Form class and children's word associations , 1966 .

[20]  Michael D. Lee,et al.  A Simple Method for Generating Additive Clustering Models with Limited Complexity , 2002, Machine Learning.

[21]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Global-Before-Basic Object Categorization in Connectionist Networks and 2-Month-Old Infants. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[22]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children's memory: An interpretive review , 1987 .

[23]  G. Murphy,et al.  Thematic relations in adults' concepts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  C. Mallows,et al.  A Method for Comparing Two Hierarchical Clusterings , 1983 .

[25]  John A. Hartigan,et al.  Clustering Algorithms , 1975 .

[26]  Eva Belke,et al.  Top-down effects of semantic knowledge in visual search are modulated by cognitive but not perceptual load , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Naming the Animals that Come to Mind: Effects of Culture and Experience on Category Fluency , 2010 .

[28]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[29]  Karrie E. Godwin,et al.  Development of inductive generalization with familiar categories , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  R. Goldstone An efficient method for obtaining similarity data , 1994 .

[31]  T. Prescott,et al.  Continuity and change in the development of category structure: Insights from the semantic fluency task , 2003 .

[32]  Michael N. Jones,et al.  Foraging in Semantic Fields: How We Search Through Memory , 2015, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[33]  Peter M. Todd,et al.  Learning and connectionist representations , 1993 .

[34]  D. Entwisle,et al.  The syntactic-paradigmatic shift in children's word associations , 1964 .

[35]  Katherine Nelson,et al.  Taxonomic Knowledge: What Kind and When? , 1992 .

[36]  Graham Thompson,et al.  The Role of Semantic Clustering in Optimal Memory Foraging , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[37]  K. Nelson,et al.  The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: a review of research and theory. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  Layla Unger,et al.  Development of category-based induction and semantic knowledge. , 2015, Child development.

[39]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[40]  S. P. Nguyen,et al.  Cross-classification and category representation in children's concepts. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[41]  S. Waxman,et al.  Challenging the notion of a thematic preference in young children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[42]  Michael C. Hout,et al.  The versatility of SpAM: a fast, efficient, spatial method of data collection for multidimensional scaling. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  D. Howard,et al.  A Multidimensional Scaling Analysis of the Development of Animal Names. , 1977 .

[44]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Charles Kemp,et al.  The discovery of structural form , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Catherine Monnier,et al.  The semantic-similarity effect in children: influence of long-term knowledge on verbal short-term memory. , 2011, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[47]  G. Murphy,et al.  Causes of taxonomic sorting by adults: A test of the thematic-to-taxonomic shift , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Will Lowe What is the Dimensionality of Human Semantic Space? , 2000, NCPW.