Performance monitoring and the causal attribution of errors
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. C. Wood,et al. Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[2] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.
[3] Thérèse J. M. Overbeek,et al. Dissociable Components of Error Processing on the Functional Significance of the Pe Vis-à-vis the Ern/ne Performance Monitoring Processes Reflected in the Ne and Pe Review of Studies That Report Both Ne and Pe: Associations and Dissociations Pharmacological Effects , 2022 .
[4] W. Gehring,et al. Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] N. Yeung,et al. Decision Processes in Human Performance Monitoring , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[6] H Stanislaw,et al. Calculation of signal detection theory measures , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[7] Mario Mikulincer,et al. Cognitive interference and learned helplessness: The effects of off-task cognitions on performance following unsolvable problems. , 1989 .
[8] Wim Fias,et al. Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[9] Barak A. Pearlmutter,et al. Linear Spatial Integration for Single-Trial Detection in Encephalography , 2002, NeuroImage.
[10] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[11] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[12] A. Engel,et al. Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Error-related brain activity and adjustments of selective attention following errors , 2011, NeuroImage.
[14] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[15] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[16] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Errors, Conflicts, and the Brain , 2004 .
[17] Wouter Hulstijn,et al. Action monitoring in motor control: ERPs following selection and execution errors in a force production task. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[18] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.
[19] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Is the Error-related Negativity Amplitude Related to Error Detectability? Evidence from Effects of Different Error Types , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] M. Mikulincer,et al. Learned helplessness, reactance, and cue utilization , 1989 .
[21] F. Heider. The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .
[22] C. Brunia,et al. Psychophysiological brain research. , 1993 .
[23] Peter Ullsperger,et al. Dissociable medial frontal negativities from a common monitoring system for self- and externally caused failure of goal achievement , 2009, NeuroImage.
[24] Dirk J. Heslenfeld,et al. Adaptive control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The role of error processing , 2007, Psychiatry Research.
[25] P. Rabbitt. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[26] Carolin Dudschig,et al. Short Article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[27] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[28] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[29] Franziska M. Korb,et al. Post-Error Behavioral Adjustments Are Facilitated by Activation and Suppression of Task-Relevant and Task-Irrelevant Information Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[30] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[31] K. Rothermund,et al. Intentional self-development: exploring the interfaces between development, intentionality, and the self. , 2002, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.
[32] David Goodman,et al. Performance Monitoring in the Anterior Cingulate is Not All Error Related: Expectancy Deviation and the Representation of Action-Outcome Associations , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[33] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[34] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[35] B. Weiner. An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. , 1985, Psychological review.
[36] Wim Fias,et al. Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.
[37] Phil Reed,et al. Interference with Judgments of Control and Attentional Shift as a Result of Prior Exposure to Controllable and Uncontrollable Feedback. , 2007 .
[38] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[39] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[40] Lucas C. Parra,et al. Recipes for the linear analysis of EEG , 2005, NeuroImage.