Inhibition in Superior Colliculus Neurons in a Brightness Discrimination Task?

Simultaneous recordings were collected from between two and four buildup neurons from the left and right superior colliculi in rhesus monkeys in a simple two-choice brightness discrimination task. The monkeys were required to move their eyes to one of two response targets to indicate their decision. Neurons were identified whose receptive fields were centered on the response targets. The functional role of inhibition was examined by conditionalizing firing rate on a high versus low rate in target neurons 90 ms to 30 ms before the saccade and examining the firing rate in both contralateral and ipsilateral neurons. Two models with racing diffusion processes were fit to the behavioral data, and the same analysis was performed on simulated paths in the diffusion processes that have been found to represent firing rate. The results produce converging evidence for the lack of a functional role for inhibition between neural populations corresponding to the two decisions.

[1]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Stochastic Dynamic Models of Response Time and Accuracy: A Foundational Primer. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[3]  M. Segraves,et al.  Muscimol-induced inactivation of monkey frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided saccades. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[5]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  A. Opstal,et al.  A nonlinear model for collicular spatial interactions underlying the metrical properties of electrically elicited saccades , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. IV. Relation of substantia nigra to superior colliculus. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Substantia nigra stimulation influences monkey superior colliculus neuronal activity bilaterally. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Perceptual Decisions between Multiple Directions of Visual Motion , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[12]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[13]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[14]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Robert H Wurtz,et al.  Modulation of presaccadic activity in the frontal eye field by the superior colliculus. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  W T Newsome,et al.  Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[19]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Signal transformations from cerebral cortex to superior colliculus for the generation of saccades , 2001, Vision Research.

[20]  Christopher D. Carello,et al.  Manipulating Intent Evidence for a Causal Role of the Superior Colliculus in Target Selection , 2004, Neuron.

[21]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  J. Hirsch,et al.  A Neural Representation of Categorization Uncertainty in the Human Brain , 2006, Neuron.

[26]  Y. Shinoda,et al.  Commissural mirror-symmetric excitation and reciprocal inhibition between the two superior colliculi and their roles in vertical and horizontal eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Neural Representation of Task Difficulty and Decision Making during Perceptual Categorization: A Timing Diagram , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  W. C. Hall,et al.  An In Vitro Study of Horizontal Connections in the Intermediate Layer of the Superior Colliculus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Monkey posterior parietal cortex neurons antidromically activated from superior colliculus. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Limits to the temporal fidelity of cortical spike rate signals , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Byounghoon Kim,et al.  Transient pauses in delay-period activity of superior colliculus neurons. , 2006, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[33]  R J Ilmoniemi,et al.  Spatiotemporal activity of a cortical network for processing visual motion revealed by MEG and fMRI. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Bair,et al.  Correlated Firing in Macaque Visual Area MT: Time Scales and Relationship to Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.

[38]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. , 1999, Psychological review.

[39]  J. Schall Neural correlates of decision processes: neural and mental chronometry , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. I. Response characteristics. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Paul Sajda,et al.  Quality of evidence for perceptual decision making is indexed by trial-to-trial variability of the EEG , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Inactivation of primate superior colliculus biases target choice for smooth pursuit, saccades, and button press responses. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Interaction of the frontal eye field and superior colliculus for saccade generation. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Unix-based multiple-process system, for real-time data acquisition and control , 1982 .

[45]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[46]  R. Ratcliff Modeling response signal and response time data , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Progression in neuronal processing for saccadic eye movements from parietal cortex area lip to superior colliculus. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[49]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Diffusive Information Accumulation by Minimal Recurrent Neural Models of Decision Making , 2011, Neural Computation.

[50]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Functional properties of corticotectal neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[52]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[53]  M. Segraves,et al.  Macaque frontal eye field input to saccade-related neurons in the superior colliculus. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[55]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Composition and topographic organization of signals sent from the frontal eye field to the superior colliculus. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Comparison of cortico-cortical and cortico-collicular signals for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[58]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Flexible Control of Mutual Inhibition: A Neural Model of Two-Interval Discrimination , 2005, Science.

[59]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Stochastic models of decisions about motion direction: Behavior and physiology , 2006, Neural Networks.

[60]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.