Human face perception traced by magneto- and electro-encephalography.

The temporal and spatial processing of face perception in normal subjects was traced by magnetoencephalography (MEG) and electroencephalography (EEG). We used 5 different visual stimuli: (1) face with opened eyes, (2) face with closed eyes, (3) eyes, (4) scrambled face, and (5) hand, and they were shown in random order. Subjects were asked to count the number of hand stimuli. To analyze the complicated brain responses to visual stimuli, we used brain electric source analysis (BESA) as the spatio-temporal multiple source model. In MEG recording, the 1M and 2M components were identified in all subjects. The 1M component was recorded to all kinds of stimuli. The 2M component was clearly identified only to face stimulation in all subjects, but to eyes stimulation in only 3 subjects with a small amplitude. The 2M component was not identified to scrambled face nor hand stimulation. The 2M component was recorded from the right hemisphere in all subjects, but in only 5 of 10 subjects from the left hemisphere. The mean peak latencies of the 1M and 2M components were approximately 132 and 179 ms, respectively. The interpeak latency between 1M and 2M was approximately 47 ms on average but the interindividual difference was large. There was no significant difference of the 2M latency between face with opened eyes and face with closed eyes. The 1M component was generated in the primary visual cortex in the bilateral hemispheres, and the 2M component was generated in the inferior temporal cortex, around the fusiform gyrus. In the EEG recording, face-specific components, positive at the vertex, P200 (Cz), and the negative at the temporal areas, N190 (T5') and N190 (T6'), were clearly recorded. The EEG results were fundamentally compatible with the MEG results. The amplitude of the component recorded from the right hemisphere was significantly larger than that from the left hemisphere. These findings suggest that the fusiform gyrus is considered to play an important role in face perception in humans, and that the right hemisphere is more dominant. Face perception takes place approximately 47 ms after the primary response to visual stimulation in the primary visual cortex, but the period of information transfer to the fusiform gyrus is variable among subjects. Detailed temporal and spatial analyses of the processing of face perception can be achieved with MEG.

[1]  H Begleiter,et al.  Event-related brain potentials differentiate priming and recognition to familiar and unfamiliar faces. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  J. C. Meadows The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia , 1974, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[3]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 2. Depth-recorded potentials in the human frontal and Rolandic cortices , 1994, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[5]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural Activation during Covert Processing of Positive Emotional Facial Expressions , 1996, NeuroImage.

[8]  R Kakigi,et al.  Visual evoked cortical magnetic fields to pattern reversal stimulation. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  R. Srebro Localization of cortical activity associated with visual recognition in humans. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual neurones responsive to faces , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  B. A. Baldwin,et al.  Cells in temporal cortex of conscious sheep can respond preferentially to the sight of faces. , 1987, Science.

[12]  T. Allison,et al.  Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Seeing faces activates three separate areas outside the occipital visual cortex in man , 1991, Neuroscience.

[14]  D. Paré,et al.  Bursting and oscillating neurons of the cat basolateral amygdaloid complex in vivo: electrophysiological properties and morphological features. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  E. Renzi,et al.  Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  T. Allison,et al.  Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers, and colors. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  J. Sergent,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  D. Perrett,et al.  Time course of neural responses discriminating different views of the face and head. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Sachiko Koyama,et al.  Reading of Japanese Kanji (morphograms) and Kana (syllabograms): a magnetoencephalographic study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  T. Allison,et al.  Comparison of cortical activation evoked by faces measured by intracranial field potentials and functional MRI: Two case studies , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[22]  C. Michel,et al.  Evidence for rapid face recognition from human scalp and intracranial electrodes , 1997, Neuroreport.

[23]  R Hari,et al.  Face-specific responses from the human inferior occipito-temporal cortex , 1997, Neuroscience.

[24]  D I Perrett,et al.  Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  T. Allison,et al.  Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Davidoff,et al.  Brain events related to normal and moderately scrambled faces. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  E. Renzi,et al.  Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined to the right hemisphere—An MRI and PET study and a review of the literature , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  D. Benson,et al.  Prosopagnosia: a bihemispheric disorder. , 1992 .

[30]  J G Ojemann,et al.  Neuronal activity related to faces and matching in human right nondominant temporal cortex. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  J. Sarvas Basic mathematical and electromagnetic concepts of the biomagnetic inverse problem. , 1987, Physics in medicine and biology.

[32]  H. Begleiter,et al.  Event-related potentials to faces: the effects of priming and recognition. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[33]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Selectively distributed processing of visual object recognition in the temporal and frontal lobes of the human brain , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[34]  Rolls Et Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. , 1984 .

[35]  E. Rolls,et al.  Selectivity between faces in the responses of a population of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[36]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Haxby,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, NeuroImage.

[38]  E. Warrington,et al.  Prosopagnosia: a clinical, psychological, and anatomical study of three patients. , 1977, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[39]  S. J. Swithenby,et al.  Neural processing of human faces: a magnetoencephalographic study , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Activation of Human Hippocampal Formation During Memory for Faces: A Pet Study , 1995, Cortex.

[41]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Prosopagnosia , 1982, Neurology.

[42]  Bruno Debruille,et al.  Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  R. Desimone Face-Selective Cells in the Temporal Cortex of Monkeys , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  U Klose,et al.  fMRI reveals amygdala activation to human faces in social phobics , 1998, Neuroreport.

[45]  R Kakigi,et al.  Visual evoked magnetic responses to central and peripheral stimulation: simultaneous VEP recordings. , 1998, Brain topography.

[46]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 1. Depth recorded potentials in the human occipital and parietal lobes , 1994, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[47]  T. Allison,et al.  Face-sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Minami Ito,et al.  Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 1992, Nature.

[49]  W. Sommer,et al.  Human brain potential correlates of face encoding into memory. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.