Spatial attention during saccade decisions.

Behavioral measures of decision making are usually limited to observations of decision outcomes. In the present study, we made use of the fact that oculomotor and sensory selection are closely linked to track oculomotor decision making before oculomotor responses are made. We asked participants to make a saccadic eye movement to one of two memorized target locations and observed that visual sensitivity increased at both the chosen and the nonchosen saccade target locations, with a clear bias toward the chosen target. The time course of changes in visual sensitivity was related to saccadic latency, with the competition between the chosen and nonchosen targets resolved faster before short-latency saccades. On error trials, we observed an increased competition between the chosen and nonchosen targets. Moreover, oculomotor selection and visual sensitivity were influenced by top-down and bottom-up factors as well as by selection history and predicted the direction of saccades. Our findings demonstrate that saccade decisions have direct visual consequences and show that decision making can be traced in the human oculomotor system well before choices are made. Our results also indicate a strong association between decision making, saccade target selection, and visual sensitivity.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We show that saccadic decisions can be tracked by measuring spatial attention. Spatial attention is allocated in parallel to the two competing saccade targets, and the time course of spatial attention differs for fast-slow and for correct-erroneous decisions. Saccade decisions take the form of a competition between potential saccade goals, which is associated with spatial attention allocation to those locations.

[1]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  Shintaro Funahashi,et al.  Prefrontal delay-period activity reflects the decision process of a saccade direction during a free-choice ODR task. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  M N Shadlen,et al.  Motion perception: seeing and deciding. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Oculomotor capture and Inhibition of Return: Evidence for an oculomotor suppression account of IOR , 2002, Psychological research.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Spatial attention-determined modifications in saccade trajectories. , 1995, Neuroreport.

[6]  Diego Fernandez-Duque,et al.  Cause and Effect Theories of Attention: The Role of Conceptual Metaphors , 2002 .

[7]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[8]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[9]  D. Sparks Functional properties of neurons in the monkey superior colliculus: Coupling of neuronal activity and saccade onset , 1978, Brain Research.

[10]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Determinants of saccade latency , 2011 .

[11]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Byounghoon Kim,et al.  Saccade Target Selection in the Superior Colliculus: A Signal Detection Theory Approach , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Jason P. Gallivan,et al.  Action plan co-optimization reveals the parallel encoding of competing reach movements , 2015, Nature Communications.

[14]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Persistent LIP activity in memory antisaccades: working memory for a sensorimotor transformation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Craig S. Chapman,et al.  Reaching for the unknown: Multiple target encoding and real-time decision-making in a rapid reach task , 2010, Cognition.

[16]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[17]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.

[18]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area Predicts the Goal and Latency of Saccades in a Free-Viewing Visual Search Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  W. Newsome,et al.  Matching Behavior and the Representation of Value in the Parietal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[20]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.

[23]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The time course of attentional and oculomotor capture reveals a common cause. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[25]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Eye movement trajectories and what they tell us , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[27]  Pierre Pouget,et al.  Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements , 2004, Psychological research.

[28]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  The parallel programming of voluntary and reflexive saccades , 2006, Vision Research.

[29]  H. Deubel,et al.  Independent Allocation of Attention to Eye and Hand Targets in Coordinated Eye-Hand Movements , 2011, Psychological science.

[30]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[31]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. 3. Cells discharging before eye movements. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  H. Deubel The time course of presaccadic attention shifts , 2008, Psychological research.

[33]  W T Newsome,et al.  Separate signals for target selection and movement specification in the superior colliculus. , 1999, Science.

[34]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Alexander Gail,et al.  I Plan Therefore I Choose: Free-Choice Bias Due to Prior Action-Probability but Not Action-Value , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[36]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[37]  T. Sawaguchi,et al.  Monkey prefrontal neuronal activity coding the forthcoming saccade in an oculomotor delayed matching-to-sample task. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  J. Schall Neural correlates of decision processes: neural and mental chronometry , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  P. Glimcher The neurobiology of visual-saccadic decision making. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  J. Jonides,et al.  Rehearsal in spatial working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  D. Munoz,et al.  Saccadic Probability Influences Motor Preparation Signals and Time to Saccadic Initiation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[44]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[46]  Huseyin Boyaci,et al.  Estimating the glossiness transfer function induced by illumination change and testing its transitivity. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[47]  R. Andersen,et al.  Target Selection Signals for Arm Reaching in the Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  E. McSorley,et al.  Saccadic eye movements as an index of perceptual decision-making , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  D. Elliott,et al.  Movement Trajectories in the Presence of a Distracting Stimulus: Evidence for a Response Activation Model of Selective Reaching , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[50]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Dissociating oculomotor contributions to spatial and feature-based selection. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  Christian Klaes,et al.  Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans , 2011, Neuron.

[52]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[53]  Roozbeh Kiani,et al.  A neural mechanism of speed-accuracy tradeoff in macaque area LIP , 2014, eLife.

[54]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Visual and Anticipatory Bias in Three Cortical Eye Fields of the Monkey during an Adaptive Decision-Making Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visual and oculomotor selection: links, causes and implications for spatial attention , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[57]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  Y. Petrov,et al.  Crowding is directed to the fovea and preserves only feature contrast. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[59]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Activity of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey during an antisaccade task , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  A. Treisman,et al.  Binding in short-term visual memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[61]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[62]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[63]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Attention allocation before antisaccades. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[64]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Movement selection in advance of action in the superior colliculus , 1992, Nature.

[65]  Brian A Anderson,et al.  Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: Testing a two-process model , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[66]  M. Chun Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  Daniel Baldauf,et al.  The Posterior Parietal Cortex Encodes in Parallel Both Goals for Double-Reach Sequences , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  Arvid Herwig,et al.  On the spatial interaction of visual working memory and attention: evidence for a global effect from memory-guided saccades. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[69]  Ulrich Ansorge,et al.  Feature-based effects in the coupling between attention and saccades. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[70]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Attention on our mind: the role of spatial attention in visual working memory. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[71]  T. Vilis,et al.  Directional selectivity of BOLD activity in human posterior parietal cortex for memory-guided double-step saccades. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[73]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[74]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[75]  E. Keller,et al.  Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus during a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[76]  P. Cisek,et al.  Decisions in Changing Conditions: The Urgency-Gating Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[77]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[79]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Altering Spatial Priority Maps via Reward-Based Learning , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[81]  Anil Bollimunta,et al.  Attention as an effect not a cause , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  P. King-Smith,et al.  Efficient and unbiased modifications of the QUEST threshold method: Theory, simulations, experimental evaluation and practical implementation , 1994, Vision Research.

[83]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role for Subthalamic Nucleus Neurons in Switching from Automatic to Controlled Eye Movement , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[84]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[85]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons , 2000, Vision Research.

[86]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Allocation of attention across saccades. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[87]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[88]  H. Lau,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity over Motor Cortex Reflects Perceptual Expectations , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[89]  T. Hodgson,et al.  The saccadic Stroop effect: Evidence for involuntary programming of eye movements by linguistic cues , 2009, Vision Research.

[90]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Target selection in visual search as revealed by movement trajectories , 2008, Vision Research.

[91]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Perceptual enhancement prior to intended and involuntary saccades. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[92]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[93]  R. Ptak The Frontoparietal Attention Network of the Human Brain , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[94]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[95]  J. Gold,et al.  The Influence of Behavioral Context on the Representation of a Perceptual Decision in Developing Oculomotor Commands , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[96]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[97]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[98]  Eric Castet,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[99]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[100]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Interactions between working memory, attention and eye movements. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[101]  Cristina Massen,et al.  Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the antisaccade task , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[102]  Eric Castet,et al.  Dynamics of attentional deployment during saccadic programming. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[103]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intention, Action Planning, and Decision Making in Parietal-Frontal Circuits , 2009, Neuron.

[104]  S. Yantis,et al.  Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[105]  T. Egner,et al.  Working memory as internal attention: Toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[106]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[107]  R. E. Morrison,et al.  Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: evidence for parallel programming of saccades. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[108]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Responses of intraparietal neurons to saccadic targets and visual distractors. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[109]  J. Gold,et al.  Caudate Encodes Multiple Computations for Perceptual Decisions , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[110]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[111]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[112]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.